08.06.2015 Views

Abstracts - Association for Chemoreception Sciences

Abstracts - Association for Chemoreception Sciences

Abstracts - Association for Chemoreception Sciences

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

P O S T E R S<br />

#P110 POSTER SESSION III: OLFACTORY<br />

PERCEPTION, HUMAN PSYCHOPHYSICS &<br />

ANIMAL BEHAVIOR; PERIPHERAL TASTE<br />

DEVELOPMENT & SIGNALING<br />

Perfume Masculinity/Femininity Affects Face Gender<br />

Judgments<br />

Theresa White 1.2<br />

1<br />

Le Moyne College Syracuse, NY, USA, 2 SUNY Upstate Medical<br />

University Syracuse, NY, USA<br />

The human face provides a great deal of in<strong>for</strong>mation that is<br />

valuable <strong>for</strong> social situations. When in<strong>for</strong>mation from a face is<br />

ambiguous, however, people look to the immediate environmental<br />

context <strong>for</strong> cues, including odors, that may help to clarify the<br />

situation. Biologically relevant odors can influence decisions<br />

about gender (Kovacs et al, 2004; Zhou & Chen, 2009) and odor<br />

valence can influence social preferences (Li et al, 2007). The<br />

present experiment asked whether the presence of commercially<br />

available perfumes (both marketed and evaluated <strong>for</strong><br />

masculinity/femininity) would provide in<strong>for</strong>mation that might<br />

influence the classification of gender in faces that were either<br />

gender distinct (male or female), or ambiguous. Male participants<br />

were divided into two groups: one of which sniffed a masculine<br />

perfume (Caesars Man, Caesars) while classifying two 8-trial<br />

blocks of faces as “male” or “female” as quickly and accurately as<br />

possible, and the other of which sniffed a feminine perfume<br />

(Shania, Stetson) while per<strong>for</strong>ming the same activity. Results<br />

showed that although the frequency of classification of ambiguous<br />

faces was not influenced by the presence of perfume, men were<br />

slower to classify an ambiguous face in a way that was<br />

inconsistent with the gender denoted by the perfume, t(16)=2.55,<br />

p=0.03. Although a ceiling effect was present <strong>for</strong> the classification<br />

of the gender distinct faces, errors tended to be made with faces<br />

that were inconsistent with the perfume that participants were<br />

given, t(17)=2.05, p=0.03, one-tailed. These results suggest that the<br />

masculinity/femininity of a fine fragrance influences decisions<br />

regarding gender, and that perfume acts as a relevant cue in<br />

guiding social interactions.<br />

#P111 POSTER SESSION III: OLFACTORY<br />

PERCEPTION, HUMAN PSYCHOPHYSICS &<br />

ANIMAL BEHAVIOR; PERIPHERAL TASTE<br />

DEVELOPMENT & SIGNALING<br />

Olfactory Brown<br />

Tali Weiss, Kobi Snitz, Elad Schneidman, Noam Sobel<br />

Department of Neurobiology, Weizmann Institute Rehovot, Israel<br />

In audition, a sound that contains every frequency within the<br />

range of human hearing, each at equal amplitude, is perceived as a<br />

hum termed “white noise”. In vision, a mixture that combines all<br />

wavelengths in the visible spectrum at equal energy is perceived as<br />

“white”, and a mixture of primary paint colors is perceived as<br />

“brown”. In fact, a careful mixture of just three primary paints,<br />

red; blue; and yellow, is sufficient to generate a brown. Here we<br />

set out to ask whether we could similarly generate an “olfactory<br />

brown”. We took advantage of the recent development of an<br />

olfactory perceptual metric space from which we could select<br />

odorants that span perception. Critically, we first diluted those<br />

odorants to equal intensity. We then found that discrimination<br />

accuracy between one 28-component mixture and another 28-<br />

component mixture was equal to discrimination accuracy of a 14-<br />

component mixture from the 28-component mixture. More<br />

critically, discrimination difficulty, and hence similarity, between<br />

the 14-component and 28-component mixture was not different<br />

from that of one 28-component mixture and another. These<br />

results suggest that mixtures containing large numbers of different<br />

odors that span perceptual olfactory space may generate a similar<br />

percept: olfactory brown. These results reflect an initial<br />

exploration of olfactory perceptual space. Due to the mammoth<br />

ef<strong>for</strong>t of equating intensity and the endless combinatorial<br />

possibilities in exploring this question, the current results should<br />

be considered a pilot ef<strong>for</strong>t only. That said, this pilot suggests that<br />

mixtures containing large numbers of different odorants that span<br />

perceptual olfactory space, and that critically were equated <strong>for</strong><br />

intensity, may generate a similar percept: olfactory brown.<br />

#P112 POSTER SESSION III: OLFACTORY<br />

PERCEPTION, HUMAN PSYCHOPHYSICS &<br />

ANIMAL BEHAVIOR; PERIPHERAL TASTE<br />

DEVELOPMENT & SIGNALING<br />

Influence of Odor Pleasantness on Perceived Intensity in<br />

Binary Mixtures<br />

Miki Wakamatsu, Yukio Sone, Hisanori Nagata, Hiroki Shikata,<br />

Yuichi Furudono<br />

Tobacco Science Research Center, Japan Tobacco Inc. Kanagawa,<br />

Japan<br />

It has been reported that human subjects tend to estimate the<br />

intensity of unpleasant odor stronger than that of pleasant odor.<br />

This result suggests odor intensity could vary according to<br />

perceived pleasantness as well as stimulus concentration, which is<br />

obviously related to intensity. On the basis of these studies, we<br />

hypothesized that if odor pleasantness is improved, this will cause<br />

the reduction of perceived intensity. To address this issue, we<br />

evaluated odor intensity and pleasantness of binary mixtures, of<br />

which pleasant odor was gradually added under the presence of<br />

constant concentration of unpleasant odor. Firstly, we asked 10<br />

human subjects to evaluate odor intensity and pleasantness as a<br />

function of concentration, and we chose 2 pleasant (dl-limonene<br />

and cycloten) and 2 unpleasant (isovaleric acid and ethanethiol)<br />

odors from these results. Then, they consecutively rated odor<br />

intensity and pleasantness of the mixture of which dl-limonene<br />

was gradually added to the constant concentration of isovaleric<br />

acid. Although odor pleasantness was improved as concentration<br />

of dl-limonene was increased, the intensity of mixture did not<br />

decrease significantly at any concentrations of added dl-limonene.<br />

However, there was the particular concentration range of dllimonene<br />

in which the pleasantness of mixture was improved<br />

without increasing the intensity. The mixture of cycloten and<br />

ethanethiol was also made and the same result was obtained.<br />

On the other hand, we could not observe such a particular<br />

concentration range in the mixture of 2 unpleasant odors. These<br />

results indicate that odor intensity of mixtures would be affected<br />

by pleasantness of components, and that unpleasantness of odor<br />

can be improved without increasing perceived intensity by adding<br />

pleasant odor in a carefully adjusted concentration.<br />

64 | AChemS <strong>Abstracts</strong> 2010 <strong>Abstracts</strong> are printed as submitted by the author(s)

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!