08.06.2015 Views

Abstracts - Association for Chemoreception Sciences

Abstracts - Association for Chemoreception Sciences

Abstracts - Association for Chemoreception Sciences

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

P O S T E R S<br />

#P122 POSTER SESSION III: OLFACTORY<br />

PERCEPTION, HUMAN PSYCHOPHYSICS &<br />

ANIMAL BEHAVIOR; PERIPHERAL TASTE<br />

DEVELOPMENT & SIGNALING<br />

Newly Discovered Specific Anosmias<br />

Charles J. Wysocki 1 , Jennifer Louie 1 , Lisa Oriolo 1 , Angelica Au 1 ,<br />

Edward Strojan 1 , Makoto Emura 2 , Michael Lankin 3<br />

1<br />

Monell Chemical Senses Center Philadelphia, PA, USA,<br />

2<br />

TAKASAGO International Corporation Hiratsuka-shi,<br />

Kanagawa, Japan, 3 TAKASAGO International Corporation<br />

Rockleigh, NJ, USA<br />

Specific anosmia is an inability to perceive an odor when others<br />

can readily detect such although the general sense of smell is<br />

intact. To date, specific anosmias <strong>for</strong> only a handful of odorants<br />

have been described. We explored human responses to 10<br />

compounds. Herein, we note that some human subjects could not<br />

smell some of the odorants, viz., 3-hydroxy-3-methylhexenoic<br />

acid, 2-nonenal, skatole and geosmin, malodorants commonly<br />

encountered by people. We determined olfactory detection<br />

thresholds <strong>for</strong> these compounds by using triangle tests (2 blanks<br />

versus the target) beginning at the lowest concentration of each<br />

(n of human subjects = 99, 177, 174 and 134, respectively). The<br />

highest concentrations were 0.1%, 0.01%, 0.01% and 0.067% and<br />

9 subsequent dilutions were 3-, 5-, 3- and 3-fold, respectively. For<br />

each triangle test the subject had to choose the odorant-containing<br />

plastic squeeze bottle (270 ml capacity containing 10 ml of diluted<br />

odorant or diluent [mineral oil]) and provide a confidence rating<br />

<strong>for</strong> the choice (3=certain; 2=possible; 1=guess). Each series was<br />

subjected to an ascending method of limits, which terminated<br />

when four correct steps were satisfied with a confidence rating<br />

summed across the last four trials of at least 7). We defined<br />

specific anosmia as reaching the highest concentration without<br />

meeting the criteria or at least 3 standard deviations above the<br />

mean. The percentage of people with specific anosmias was<br />

14.1%, 6.2%, 2.3% and 6.0% respectively. These data suggest that<br />

there may be far more specific anosmias than previously reported:<br />

In the 10 selected <strong>for</strong> in-depth study, 40% could not be detected<br />

by a subset of those tested. Perhaps these result from altered or<br />

absent gene expression. Acknowledgements: Supported by NIH<br />

RO1 DC00298 to CJW and support from TAKASAGO.<br />

#P123 POSTER SESSION III: OLFACTORY<br />

PERCEPTION, HUMAN PSYCHOPHYSICS &<br />

ANIMAL BEHAVIOR; PERIPHERAL TASTE<br />

DEVELOPMENT & SIGNALING<br />

Early Odor Learning in Tree Swallows (Tachycineta bicolor)<br />

Meredyth P. Duncan, Ashley A. Miniet, Julie C. Hagelin<br />

Department of Biology, Swarthmore College Swarthmore, PA,<br />

USA<br />

Perching birds (Passerines) have exceedingly small olfactory bulbs<br />

and their chemosensory abilities are only minimally studied. We<br />

investigated the Tree Swallow (Tachycineta bicolor), a common<br />

migratory passerine that breeds in North America. We tested:<br />

(1) whether swallow chicks could detect odors, and (2) whether<br />

exposure to an experimental odor early in life would alter a<br />

chick’s response. Our field-study involved chicks from two nest<br />

types. In experimental nests (n = 15) all chicks were exposed to<br />

the scent of peppermint oil during incubation and chick rearing,<br />

whereas those in control nests (n = 31) were not. At nine days of<br />

age we tested chicks <strong>for</strong> their reaction to peppermint oil, fox urine<br />

(a putative predator scent), and an odorless control using the<br />

Porter method (Porter et al. 1999). Chicks from both treatments<br />

detected test odors relative to the odorless control (2.28 ≤ t ≤ 3.39,<br />

0.004 ≤ P ≤ 0.031). Next, we compared the degree with which<br />

birds from each treatment discriminated between our two test<br />

scents by subtracting a chick’s response to mint from its response<br />

to fox. Chicks reared in mint odor exhibited values that were<br />

nearly twice that of controls (t = 2.05, df = 44, P = 0.046),<br />

suggesting that early exposure to mint altered a bird’s ability to<br />

differentiate between mint and fox odors. Birds in experimental<br />

boxes appeared more familiar with mint in that they reacted less<br />

strongly to mint than to the unfamiliar odor of fox. By contrast,<br />

birds from control boxes reacted similarly to mint and fox, both<br />

of which were unfamiliar odors. Our results suggest that<br />

chemosensory learning can occur early on in passerine<br />

development and we suspect this may be true of other birds.<br />

Studies of odor imprinting are a promising future step <strong>for</strong> avian<br />

research. Acknowledgements: An HHMI Undergraduate Summer<br />

Research stipend was granted to AAM. MPD was supported by a<br />

summer research grant from the Natural <strong>Sciences</strong> Division of<br />

Swarthmore College.<br />

#P124 POSTER SESSION III: OLFACTORY<br />

PERCEPTION, HUMAN PSYCHOPHYSICS &<br />

ANIMAL BEHAVIOR; PERIPHERAL TASTE<br />

DEVELOPMENT & SIGNALING<br />

Exploring the Olfactory Ability of the Kea (Nestor notabilis),<br />

an Endangered Parrot from New Zealand<br />

Kimberly F. Kramer 1 , Regina L. Kukola 2 , Christine Q. Ernst 1 ,<br />

Julie C. Hagelin 1<br />

1<br />

Department of Biology, Swarthmore College Swarthmore, PA,<br />

USA, 2 Department of Biology, Bryn Mawr College Bryn Mawr,<br />

PA, USA<br />

The Kakapo (Strigops habroptilis) is a critically endangered,<br />

endemic parrot of New Zealand known <strong>for</strong> its nocturnal habit and<br />

olfactory ability (Hagelin 2004). Most parrots, however, are<br />

diurnal and their chemosensory abilities are practically unstudied.<br />

The Kea (Nestor notabilis) offers a particularly interesting<br />

comparison, because this endangered species is a diurnal relative<br />

of the Kakapo. We studied two captive Keas at the Philadelphia<br />

Zoo to determine whether birds could detect amyl acetate (banana<br />

odor). As part of an enrichment regimen birds were given either<br />

toy balls of per<strong>for</strong>ated plastic that had been internally treated with<br />

scent or identical balls that were unscented. Pilot testing led us to<br />

focus on an unusual behavior, “face rubbing,” in which a bird<br />

wipes its cheeks, nostrils or beak against the outside of balls. Over<br />

60-minute trials the behavior of each bird was scored by an<br />

observer blind to the toy treatment. Face-rubbing was more likely<br />

to occur when birds played with scented balls (N = 21 trials,<br />

Fisher’s exact P = 0.03). Our result indicates that Keas can detect<br />

and react differently to toys treated with banana odor. Scented<br />

toys are a practical means of providing base-line in<strong>for</strong>mation<br />

about the chemosenses <strong>for</strong> many avian species. We also suggest<br />

that this method may be a potentially useful but overlooked<br />

means of enrichment <strong>for</strong> captive birds.<br />

68 | AChemS <strong>Abstracts</strong> 2010 <strong>Abstracts</strong> are printed as submitted by the author(s)

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!