08.06.2015 Views

Abstracts - Association for Chemoreception Sciences

Abstracts - Association for Chemoreception Sciences

Abstracts - Association for Chemoreception Sciences

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

P O S T E R S<br />

typically 40-minute session of 200-400 trials. After acquisition of<br />

this task novel stimuli are also presented at the manifold after<br />

which rats are rewarded at any spout of their choice. The spatial<br />

response pattern directly reflects the similarity of the novel<br />

stimulus to the acquired training set of tastants. Four rats were<br />

successfully trained, achieving 60-80% accuracy after 130 days of<br />

training. Controls confirmed rats used taste cues to guide their<br />

responses. One of the rats showed deviant responses, which<br />

ultimately converged with those of the other rats. All rats showed<br />

clear concentration-response functions (0, 3,10, 33, 100 and 300%<br />

of conc. of train stimuli), which were stable across test sessions.<br />

Generalization to novel tastants was also evaluated. Rats can learn<br />

to associate tastes with locations, which can be used to investigate<br />

their perception of taste quality and intensity of novel stimuli.<br />

Acknowledgements: Supported by R01 DC009994-01<br />

#P182 POSTER SESSION IV: CHEMOSENSORY<br />

TRANSDUCTION AND SIGNALING<br />

Behavioral and anatomical characterization of sucralose<br />

preferring and avoiding rats<br />

Gregory C Loney, Ann Marie Torregrossa, Lisa A Eckel<br />

Florida State University Tallahassee, FL, USA<br />

Rats display a bimodal preference <strong>for</strong> the non-nutritive sweetener<br />

sucralose. While some prefer sucralose over water across a range<br />

of concentrations, others avoid sucralose at concentrations above<br />

0.1 g/L. While this phenomenon has been studied primarily in<br />

males, there is some evidence that females may differ from males<br />

in their preference <strong>for</strong> sucralose. Currently, the mechanism<br />

underlying individual differences in sucralose preference in either<br />

sex is poorly understood, but some data suggest that it may be<br />

related to individual differences in sensitivity to a bitter taste<br />

quality of sucralose. In humans, increased sensitivity to bitter<br />

substances appears to be positively correlated with the number of<br />

fungi<strong>for</strong>m papillae counted on the surface of the tongue. The goal<br />

of the present study was to determine whether preference <strong>for</strong><br />

sucralose is influenced by sex and/or the number of fungi<strong>for</strong>m<br />

papillae on the rat’s tongue. Male and female rats (n=22/sex) were<br />

given access to ascending concentrations of sucralose (0.0001 - 2.0<br />

g/L) and water in two-bottle, 24-h preference tests. While a<br />

greater proportion of males than females were characterized as<br />

sucralose preferrers (40% vs 30%), once categorized, we found no<br />

sex difference in their preference curves. The tongues of a subset<br />

of these rats (10 preferrers; 11 avoiders) were incised just anterior<br />

to the median molar eminence, stained with methylene blue, and<br />

the number of fungi<strong>for</strong>m papillae with clear taste pores was<br />

counted. Sucralose avoiders had significantly more papillae than<br />

did sucralose preferrers (109±4 vs 95±5, respectively, t(19)=2.18,<br />

p

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!