08.06.2015 Views

Abstracts - Association for Chemoreception Sciences

Abstracts - Association for Chemoreception Sciences

Abstracts - Association for Chemoreception Sciences

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

#P325 POSTER SESSION VII:<br />

OLFACTORY PSYCHOPHYSICS &<br />

CLINICAL STUDIES; CENTRAL OLFACTION<br />

Eruction Sensitive Subjective Hypogeusia<br />

Alan R. Hirsch<br />

Smell & Taste Treatment and Reasearch Foundation Chicago, IL,<br />

USA<br />

OBJECTIVE: To illustrate the importance of retronasal olfaction<br />

<strong>for</strong> perception of taste. BACKGROUND: Physiologic<br />

synesthesia with taste/smell confusion is the chemosensory<br />

underpinning of perceived flavor which is mediated through<br />

retronasal olfaction. The importance of this mechanism in the<br />

perception of flavor is manifest in the case study.<br />

METHODS/RESULTS: 70 year-old retired teacher was nasute<br />

until one year prior to presentation when, after a severe upper<br />

respiratory infection, suddenly lost all smell, with gradual<br />

recovery of approximately 30%. Concurrent with the smell loss<br />

was a perceived decreased ability to taste all but sweet, sour, and<br />

spicy, whereas all foods tasted flavorless. With eructation, totally<br />

normal flavor would return (ie. chewing what tasted like<br />

cardboard, on eructation, would suddenly trans<strong>for</strong>m to garlic<br />

bread, rubber to cantaloupe, etc.). Otolaryngolgic and nasal<br />

fiberoptic endoscopic examinations and CT of the head and<br />

sinuses were normal. Chemosensory testing suggested<br />

normogeusia on quadrant testing and Accusens Taste test, but<br />

severe olfactory deficit: right and left unilateral PEA threshold<br />

testing greater than -2.0; UPSIT left nostril 16, right nostril<br />

10; Sniffin’ Stick Threshold unilaterally and dirhinously less than<br />

1; discrimination left 3, right 5, dirhinously 6; identification left 8,<br />

right 9, dirhinously 7; odor memory test 2 at ten seconds, 3 at<br />

thirty seconds, and 1 at sixty seconds.<br />

CONCLUSIONS/RELEVANCE: The perceived normogeusia<br />

upon eructation in this otherwise subjectively hypogeusic<br />

individual demonstrated the importance of retronasal olfaction in<br />

the perception of flavor. SOURCE OF FUNDING: None<br />

#P326 POSTER SESSION VII:<br />

OLFACTORY PSYCHOPHYSICS &<br />

CLINICAL STUDIES; CENTRAL OLFACTION<br />

Identification of odor active substances in human<br />

amniotic fluid<br />

Constanze Hartmann 1 , Sébastien Doucet 1,2,3 , Ralf Dittrich 4 ,<br />

Benoist Schaal 3 , Andrea Buettner 1,2<br />

1<br />

University of Erlangen-Nuremberg, Department of Chemistry<br />

and Pharmacy Erlangen, Germany, 2 Fraunhofer Institute <strong>for</strong><br />

Process Engineering and Packaging Freising, Germany, 3 Centre<br />

des <strong>Sciences</strong> du Gout, Developmental Ethology and Cognitive<br />

Psychology Group Dijon, France, 4 University of Erlangen-<br />

Nuremberg, University Hospital Erlangen Erlangen, Germany<br />

Objectives. Physiological evidence indicates that olfaction could<br />

already function in the human fetus [1]. Right after birth the odor<br />

of amniotic fluid (AF) is detectable to newborns and seems to<br />

attract [2] and calm them [3]. Such early attraction may derive<br />

from fetal learning/memory, and may help newborns adapt to the<br />

postnatal environment. The chemosensory basis of AF<br />

attractiveness has not been investigated yet, and this study aimed<br />

to characterize odor active compounds therein. Methods. The<br />

flavor profile of AF was monitored by descriptive sensory<br />

evaluation using an adult panel, while the identification of the<br />

predominant odor substances was carried out by senso-analytical<br />

techniques like gas chromatography-olfactometry and<br />

comparative dilution assays. Results. The prevailing AF odor<br />

impressions were described as blood- and raw meat-like. Several<br />

odor active substances were identified, amongst them some<br />

carbonyl compounds, as well as androstenone. Conclusions.<br />

Human AF contains a wide range of odorous substances, such as<br />

odor active steroids or ketones. These substances could have the<br />

potential ability to promote attention and hush the newborn, <strong>for</strong><br />

example during noxious medical examination. References. [1]<br />

Schaal et al. 2004. Clin Perinatol 31:261-81 [2] Schaal et al. 1995.<br />

Biol Neonate 67, 397-406. [3] Varendi et al 1998. Early Hum Dev<br />

51:47-55. Acknowledgements: Financed by the German Federal<br />

Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF) and the Bavarian<br />

Research Foundation.<br />

#P327 POSTER SESSION VII:<br />

OLFACTORY PSYCHOPHYSICS &<br />

CLINICAL STUDIES; CENTRAL OLFACTION<br />

Human Neonatal Responses to Androstenone<br />

Sebastien Doucet 1,2,3 , Constanze Hartmann 2 , Ralph Dittrich 4 ,<br />

Robert Soussignan 1 , Benoist Schaal 1 , Andrea Buettner 2,3<br />

1<br />

Developmental Ethology and Cognitive Psychology Group,<br />

Centre des <strong>Sciences</strong> du Goût, CNRS Dijon, France, 2 Department<br />

of Chemistry and Pharmacy, Emil Fischer Center, University of<br />

Erlangen-Nuremberg Erlangen, Germany, 3 Sensory Analytics,<br />

Fraunhofer Institute <strong>for</strong> Process Engineering and Packaging IVV<br />

Freising, Germany, 4 University-Hospital Erlangen, University of<br />

Erlangen-Nuremberg Erlangen, Germany<br />

Aims. Human newborns show equal attraction to the odors of<br />

amniotic fluid (AF) and mothers’ colostrum [1]. As 5-a-androst-<br />

16-en-3-one (An) has been found in both fluids [2, 3], we studied<br />

it as a potential vector of this perinatal odor continuity and of<br />

neonatal attraction to the smell of breast and milk [4].<br />

Methods. Two studies were run. 1) We recorded the behavior of<br />

16 newborns (3 days) and 26 adults exposed to a saturating water<br />

solution of An and to 3 reference odorants [butyric acid, B;<br />

vanilla, V; water, W]. 2) We videotaped 26 newborns (3 days)<br />

being administered 7 stimuli: 4 dilution steps of An and 3<br />

reference odorants [familiar milk, V, W]. Oral and facial actions to<br />

each odorant were quantified in infants, while verbal responses<br />

were recorded in adults. Results. In study 1, 12/16 neonates<br />

reacted with negative facial actions to An, while 10, 3 and 2<br />

responded in such way to B, V and W, respectively. In contrast,<br />

11/26 adults rated An as unpleasant, while 26 rated B as negative,<br />

and V and W as positive/neutral. In study 2, 24/26 newborns<br />

increased the relative duration of negative facial actions and/or<br />

decreased the duration of oro-cephalic movements when exposed<br />

to An as compared to W. Conclusions. A great majority of<br />

newborns detect An and, as adults who are osmic to An, respond<br />

to An in showing unpleasantness. Thus, the status of specific<br />

anosmia to An clearly differs in neonates and adults (<br />

50% being non responsive, respectively). Finally, the negative<br />

responsiveness to a compound met prenatally appears paradoxical<br />

when considering previous data on fetal odor learning.<br />

References. [1] Schaal B et al. (1998). Behav Neurosci, 112, 1438-<br />

49; [2] Doucet S et al., submitted; [3] Buettner A (2007). Flav<br />

Fragr J, 22, 465-73; [4] Doucet S et al. (2007). Dev Psychobiol, 49,<br />

129–38. Acknowledgements: SD was supported by Bavarian<br />

Research Foundation and Region Burgundy. CH and AB were<br />

funded by the German Federal Ministry of Education and<br />

Research (BMBF), and BS by CNRS (EAL 549).<br />

P O S T E R S<br />

<strong>Abstracts</strong> are printed as submitted by the author(s)<br />

<strong>Abstracts</strong> | 135

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!