08.06.2015 Views

Abstracts - Association for Chemoreception Sciences

Abstracts - Association for Chemoreception Sciences

Abstracts - Association for Chemoreception Sciences

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

#P119 POSTER SESSION III: OLFACTORY<br />

PERCEPTION, HUMAN PSYCHOPHYSICS &<br />

ANIMAL BEHAVIOR; PERIPHERAL TASTE<br />

DEVELOPMENT & SIGNALING<br />

Process differences between physical and physiological odor<br />

mixtures<br />

Malin Brodin 1 , Simona Negoias 2 , Mats J Olsson 1<br />

1<br />

Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet<br />

Stockholm, Sweden, 2 Smell & Taste Clinic, University of Dresden<br />

Medical School Dresden, Germany<br />

We investigated whether a mixture will be perceived differently if<br />

it has been presented as a mixture in one nostril, in comparison to<br />

if two odorants are presented simultaneously in separate nostrils.<br />

In parallel, we investigated whether the different types of<br />

presentation gave rise to differences in event related potentials<br />

(ERP). Twenty-four healthy persons, 12 men and12 women,<br />

between the ages of 18 to 35 years participated in the study. Using<br />

the Sniffin’ Sticks threshold test, all participants were screened <strong>for</strong><br />

threshold differences between both nostrils separately. We<br />

excluded those with threshold differences over 2 points between<br />

the two nostrils (Gudziol et al, 2006). We compared single<br />

odorants A or B monorhinally, mixtures of A and B<br />

monorhinally, and simultaneous presentation of A and B<br />

birhinally. The odorants used were eugenol (A) and l-carvone (B).<br />

The stimuli were presented, using a computer-controlled airdilution<br />

olfactometer (OM6B; Burghart instruments, Wedel,<br />

Germany), in a constant flow of odorless and humidified air of<br />

controlled temperature (250 ms stimulus duration; 80% relative<br />

humidity; total flow of 7 L/min; 36 degrees C). Participants were<br />

asked to rate the composition of the stimuli on a visual analogue<br />

scale. Hence, they rated to which extent they perceived a single<br />

odorant, A or B, or a mixture. The participants also rated the<br />

overall intensity of the stimuli. Additionally, ERPs <strong>for</strong> each type<br />

of stimuli were recorded. Analyses will focus on the differences<br />

between monorhinal and birhinal mixtures. Hence, both the<br />

differences in the ERPs and in the psychophysical ratings will be<br />

analyzed. The implications of potential difference between these<br />

physical and physiological mixtures <strong>for</strong> the understanding of<br />

mixture processing will be discussed.<br />

#P120 POSTER SESSION III: OLFACTORY<br />

PERCEPTION, HUMAN PSYCHOPHYSICS &<br />

ANIMAL BEHAVIOR; PERIPHERAL TASTE<br />

DEVELOPMENT & SIGNALING<br />

Rapid yet short-lived olfactory plasticity in wake and in sleep<br />

Anat Arzi, Noam Sobel<br />

Weizmann Institute of Science Rehovot, Israel<br />

occasions: in the morning, 7 hours later, and one day later. A<br />

control group of 18 female subjects participated in the 3 threshold<br />

tests only. We found a significant decrease in thresholds in the<br />

exposure group (M =11.55± 0.4 in the first test, M = 10.8± 0.4 in<br />

the second test, p > 0.002) but not in the control group (M =<br />

10.75± 0.3 in the first test, M = 10.02± 0.5 in the second test, p =<br />

0.27). This plasticity, however, was short lived, as thresholds<br />

returned to baseline the next day (Exposure group: M = 11.42±<br />

0.5 in the third test, p = 0.66; Control group: 10.3± 0.4 in the third<br />

test, p = 0.3). That functional plasticity occurs at such short timeframes<br />

may help pinpoint the neural substrates of this behavioral<br />

change. Finally, we are currently replicating this study during<br />

sleep in order to assess the role of conscious perception in the<br />

plasticity process.<br />

#P121 POSTER SESSION III: OLFACTORY<br />

PERCEPTION, HUMAN PSYCHOPHYSICS &<br />

ANIMAL BEHAVIOR; PERIPHERAL TASTE<br />

DEVELOPMENT & SIGNALING<br />

Effect Of Eye Closure On Olfactory Detection Threshold<br />

Amit K Bhise 1 , Alan R. Hirsch 2 , Amal Asiri 2<br />

1<br />

Sinia Hospital of Baltimore, Baltimore, MD, USA,<br />

2<br />

Smell and Taste Treatment and Research Foundation,<br />

Chicago, IL, USA<br />

Objective: To determine the effect of eye closure on olfactory<br />

detection threshold. Introduction: Part of the evaluation of cranial<br />

nerve I is the assessment of olfactory threshold. Smell Threshold<br />

Test (STT) of Doty measures olfactory detection threshold<br />

(ODT) to phenyl-ethyl alcohol (PEA). Currently, the effect of<br />

visual stimuli on olfaction is unclear. Methods: Ten healthy,<br />

subjectively normasmic volunteers (7 women, 3 men), mean age<br />

29 years (range 18-57 year) underwent STT based upon single<br />

staircase-<strong>for</strong>ced choice paradigm. Subjects kept their eyes open<br />

and then closed, or vice-versa, as ODT was determined by<br />

presenting them with varying concentration of PEA ( -10.0 to -2.0<br />

log units) in half log steps. Order of testing alternated after each<br />

subject, so that one half had odor testing with eyes closed first,<br />

while the other half had testing with eyes open first. A 20 minute<br />

washout period existed between the tests in each individual. Each<br />

subject served as their own control. Result: Mean ODT with eyes<br />

open was -3.7 (range -4.6 to -2.2), and with eyes closed was -4.0<br />

(range -6.5 to -2.2). No significant difference was noted (p>0.05)<br />

in threshold values <strong>for</strong> eyes open versus eyes closed. Conclusion:<br />

We had anticipated that the absence of visual stimuli would lower<br />

the olfactory detection threshold, possibly due to reduction of<br />

competing sensory stimuli, allowing focusing of attention.<br />

However, no discernible effect was found. Sources of Funding:<br />

None<br />

P O S T E R S<br />

Several studies have demonstrated that long-term repetitions of<br />

relatively brief olfactory exposures can lead to functional<br />

plasticity reflected in reduced (improved) olfactory detection<br />

thresholds <strong>for</strong> the exposed odorant. Here we set out to ask<br />

whether we could accelerate this process using a more aggressive<br />

exposure paradigm. We measured detection thresholds <strong>for</strong> the<br />

odorant citral (CAS #5392-40-5) using 20 log dilutions (highest<br />

concentration set at 3.125 % v/v citral in mineral oil) delivered<br />

within the maximum-likelihood adaptive staircase method. An<br />

exposure group of 20 female subjects smelled the odorant <strong>for</strong> 3<br />

continuous minutes every 15 minutes <strong>for</strong> 20 times summing at one<br />

full hour of exposure. In addition, we measured threshold on 3<br />

<strong>Abstracts</strong> are printed as submitted by the author(s)<br />

<strong>Abstracts</strong> | 67

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!