08.06.2015 Views

Abstracts - Association for Chemoreception Sciences

Abstracts - Association for Chemoreception Sciences

Abstracts - Association for Chemoreception Sciences

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

PROP strips and PROP solutions. PAV/PAV and PAV/AVI<br />

participants showed a larger difference between PROP strips and<br />

control strips, relative to AVI/AVI participants <strong>for</strong> the gLMS and<br />

hedonic data. These results indicate that edible strips are suitable<br />

<strong>for</strong> examining PROP taster status in humans. Acknowledgements:<br />

Supported by federal funds from the National Institute on Aging,<br />

National Institutes of Health, Contract HHS-N-260-2006 00007-<br />

C. (PI, R. Gershon), and by NIDCD R44 DC00729<br />

#P6 POSTER SESSION I: TASTE IMAGING &<br />

PSYCHOPHYSICS; CENTRAL TASTE;<br />

MULTIPLE MODALITIES; CENTRAL &<br />

PERIPHERAL OLFACTION<br />

Differences in endogenous bitterness of Rebaudioside A do not<br />

appear to impact psychophysical compression of the sweetness<br />

power function<br />

Ellen D Mahan, Julie A Peterson, John E Hayes<br />

Department of Food Science, College of Agricultural <strong>Sciences</strong>,<br />

Penn State University Park, PA, USA<br />

Although natural high intensity sweeteners have long been a<br />

scientific curiosity, a stevia derived sweetener, Rebaudioside A<br />

(RebA), was recently granted GRAS status, resulting in renewed<br />

commercial interest and a new generation of natural sweeteners<br />

on the market. Because RebA is both bitter and sweet, variation in<br />

perceived bitterness may differentially impact RebA sweetness<br />

across individuals via mixture suppression. We speculated this<br />

would result in greater compression in the psychophysical<br />

function <strong>for</strong> RebA sweetness (eg sweetness would grow more<br />

slowly with concentration in those experiencing the most<br />

bitterness). Here, 21 subjects (5 men) tasted sucrose (1.8, 4.2, 5.6,<br />

7.5, 18% w/v) and RebA (.013, .032, .042, .056, .13 % w/v)<br />

samples in triplicate, rating sweetness and bitterness using the<br />

generalized labeled magnitude scale (gLMS). Data were analyzed<br />

with a ‘poor-man’s multilevel model’: a compound specific power<br />

function was estimated <strong>for</strong> each subject via regression, and then<br />

each subject’s power exponent was used as a variable in a<br />

subsequent regression model. As anticipated, the power<br />

exponents <strong>for</strong> sucrose and RebA were correlated across subjects<br />

(eg greater growth in sweetness with concentration). Also, a<br />

subject’s sucrose exponent was greater than their RebA sweetness<br />

exponent in all cases. Contrary to our hypothesis however, we did<br />

not find the expected negative relationship between maximal<br />

RebA bitterness and compression in a subjects’ RebA sweetness<br />

function. Instead, we found a substantial trend in the other<br />

direction, such that RebA bitterness was positively correlated<br />

with an accelerating RebA sweetness function. Collectively, this<br />

suggests sweet specific or generalized supertasting may<br />

overwhelm mixture suppression in determining the sweetness of<br />

tastants that are also bitter.<br />

#P7 POSTER SESSION I: TASTE IMAGING &<br />

PSYCHOPHYSICS; CENTRAL TASTE;<br />

MULTIPLE MODALITIES; CENTRAL &<br />

PERIPHERAL OLFACTION<br />

Neural Correlates of Self-Initiated Tasting in Humans<br />

Danielle M Douglas 1 , Maria G Veldhuizen 1,2 , John Buckley 1 ,<br />

Micheal Fritz 1 , Dana Small 1,2,3<br />

1<br />

John B. Pierce Laboratory New Haven, CT, USA, 2 Yale<br />

University School of Medicine New Haven, CT, USA,<br />

3<br />

Department of Psychology, Yale University New Haven, CT,<br />

USA<br />

Taste sensations occur primarily during the act of eating and<br />

drinking. To date, neuroimaging investigations have focused<br />

exclusively on measuring brain response to taste during passive<br />

delivery, where the subject lies and waits <strong>for</strong> a taste stimulus to<br />

arrive on their tongue or into their mouth. Recent work in<br />

animals highlights the importance of the act of obtaining a taste<br />

stimulus upon its neural representation. Moreover, it is clear that<br />

many of the regions that respond to taste in humans also<br />

orchestrate oral movement and somatosensation. The goal of the<br />

current study was to use fMRI to compare brain response to taste<br />

vs. tasteless during self-initiated vs. passive receipt. A custombuilt<br />

mouthpiece, equipped with a suction sensor that relayed<br />

signals to a computer to trigger immediate delivery, allowed<br />

subjects to self-initiate stimulus delivery. Ten subjects received<br />

tasteless and sucrose solutions during passive and self-initiated<br />

delivery. Delivery had no effect on perceptual ratings of intensity,<br />

familiarity, liking or sweetness. Imaging data were analyzed using<br />

a flexible factorial design based upon random effects models.<br />

Irrespective of stimulus, self-initiated vs. passive delivery was<br />

associated strong preferential response in bilateral insular and<br />

somatomotor mouth areas, striatum and cerebellum. Additionally,<br />

a stimulus by delivery interaction was observed in the frontal<br />

operculum, which responded maximally during sensation of a<br />

self-initiated taste. Trends towards a similar interaction were also<br />

observed in the insula and orbitofrontal cortex. These findings<br />

indicate that brain response to taste is significantly stronger when<br />

it is actively obtained and that the act of sucking is orchestrated<br />

by a network that overlaps with oral sensory areas.<br />

#P8 POSTER SESSION I: TASTE IMAGING &<br />

PSYCHOPHYSICS; CENTRAL TASTE;<br />

MULTIPLE MODALITIES; CENTRAL &<br />

PERIPHERAL OLFACTION<br />

Valid comparisons of food preferences<br />

Linda M. Bartoshuk 1 , Jaclyn J. Kalva 1 , Lorenzo A. Puentes 1 ,<br />

Derek J. Snyder 1,2 , Charles A. Sims 1<br />

1<br />

UF Center <strong>for</strong> Smell & Taste Gainesville, FL, USA, 2 Yale<br />

University New Haven, CT, USA<br />

P O S T E R S<br />

Taste comparisons are commonly made with category scales (e.g.,<br />

Natick 9-point), visual analogue scales and the Labeled Magnitude<br />

Scale, but labels on these scales generally fail to denote the same<br />

taste intensities to all, leading to invalid comparisons. To remedy<br />

this problem, we created a general Labeled Magnitude Scale<br />

(gLMS) encompassing all sensations (0=no sensation, 100=most<br />

intense sensation of any kind ever experienced). Because the most<br />

intense sensation ever perceived is rarely a taste, the top anchor of<br />

the gLMS acts as a standard <strong>for</strong> groups varying on a taste-related<br />

attribute (e.g., taste bud density), enabling valid taste<br />

<strong>Abstracts</strong> are printed as submitted by the author(s)<br />

<strong>Abstracts</strong> | 29

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!