10.04.2013 Views

Linguistics Encyclopedia.pdf

Linguistics Encyclopedia.pdf

Linguistics Encyclopedia.pdf

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

A-Z 91<br />

d. I paid Harry five dollars for a dozen roses.<br />

Faced by criticism from Anderson (1971) and Mellema (1974), Fillmore accepts that<br />

there are some semantic generalizations which can only be formulated if the grammatical<br />

relations of subject and object are recognized at some level of representation in<br />

grammatical theory. For instance, whether a noun phrase be given a holistic or a<br />

partitive interpretation seems to depend on its grammatical status as object or subject, as<br />

exemplified in (1) The garden was swarming with bees (holistic: the whole garden is<br />

swarming with bees; the garden in subject position) and (2) I loaded the truck with hay<br />

(holistic: the whole truck is loaded; the truck in object position); versus (3) Bees were<br />

swarming in the garden (partitive: the garden is not necessarily full of bees) and (4) I<br />

loaded hay onto the truck (partitive: the truck may not be full of hay).<br />

A major worry for case theory is that none of the linguists who have developed<br />

grammars in which the notion of case figures has been able to arrive at a principled way<br />

of defining the cases, or of deciding how many cases there are, or of deciding when two<br />

cases have something in common as opposed to being simply variants of one case (Cruse,<br />

1973). For example, Huddleston (1970) points out that in The wind opened the door, the<br />

wind may be interpreted as having its own energy and hence as being agent, or as being<br />

merely a direct cause of the door opening, and hence as instrument, or as having a role<br />

which is distinct from both agent and instrument, called, perhaps, ‘force’. On yet another<br />

view, a case feature ‘cause’ can be seen as a feature of both agent and instrument<br />

(Fillmore, 1977, p. 71). Fillmore thinks that this problem may be explained with<br />

reference to the notions of perspective and of meaning being relativized to scenes<br />

mentioned above. The wind is brought into perspective in the clause and is thus a nuclear<br />

element. And (pp. 79–80): ‘perspectivizing corresponds, in English, to determining the<br />

structuring of a clause in terms of the nuclear grammatical relations’.<br />

The obvious attractions of case grammar include the clear semantic relevance of<br />

notions such as agency, causation, location, advantage to someone, etc. These are easily<br />

identifiable across languages, and are held by many psychologists to play an important<br />

part in child language acquisition. However (Lyons, 1977a, pp. 87–8):<br />

case-grammar is no longer seen by the majority of linguists working<br />

within the general framework of transformational-generative grammar as<br />

a viable alternative to the standard theory. The reason is that when it<br />

comes to classifying the totality of the verbs in a language in terms of the<br />

deep-structure cases that they govern, the semantic criteria which define<br />

these cases are all too often unclear or in conflict.<br />

In spite of its failings, case grammar has been important in drawing the attention of an<br />

initially sceptical tradition of linguistic study to the importance of relating semantic cases<br />

or thematic roles to syntactic descriptions.<br />

K.M.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!