10.04.2013 Views

Linguistics Encyclopedia.pdf

Linguistics Encyclopedia.pdf

Linguistics Encyclopedia.pdf

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

The linguistics encyclopedia 154<br />

These observations range from the first reference to a dominant hemisphere of the<br />

brain for language by Broca (1865), a French neurologist (see LANGUAGE<br />

PATHOLOGY AND NEUROLINGUISTICS), to the first use of the term word<br />

blindness by Kussmaul (1877), a German internist; the term ‘dyslexia’ was first used by<br />

Professor Berlin of Stuttgart in 1887 as an alternative to ‘word blindness’. In 1892,<br />

Professor Déjérine of Paris found that in the brains of stroke patients with attendant<br />

dyslexia, the damage tended to be located in the posterior-temporal region in the left<br />

cerebral hemisphere, where the parietal and occipital lobes meet. The specialists<br />

mentioned above were in the main working with traumatized patients who suffered<br />

disturbances of spoken and written language. However, from 1895, James Hinshelwood,<br />

a Glasgow eye surgeon, published in The Lancet and The British Medical Journal a series<br />

of articles describing a similar disorder, but not apparently caused by brain injury. He<br />

described the phenomenon as<br />

a constitutional defect occurring in children with otherwise normal and<br />

undamaged brains, characterized by a disability in learning to read so<br />

great that it is manifestly due to pathological conditions and where the<br />

attempts to teach the child by ordinary methods have failed.<br />

(Hinshelwood, 1917, p. 16)<br />

Following upon Hinshelwood’s seminal work in this field, the notion of a developmental<br />

dyslexia was accepted by a number of medical and psychological authorities. These<br />

include the eminent American neurologist Samuel Orton, who, in 1937, described the<br />

underlying features of dyslexia as difficulties in acquiring series and in looking ‘at<br />

random’, associating the occurrence with unstable patterns of individual laterality. He<br />

related such patterns to hemispheric control of functions, and referred to the problem as<br />

one of ‘lacking cerebral dominance’. The neurological conception of dyslexia may be<br />

summed up in Skydgaard’s brief definition (1942): ‘A primary constitutional reading<br />

disability which may occur electively’, or at greater length in Critchley(1964, p. 5):<br />

Within the heterogeneous community of poor readers, there exists a<br />

specific syndrome wherein particular difficulty exists in learning the<br />

conventional meaning of a verbal symbol and of associating the sound<br />

with the symbol in appropriate fashion. Such cases are marked by their<br />

gravity and purity. They are ‘grave’ in that the difficulty transcends the<br />

more common backwardness in reading and the prognosis is more serious<br />

unless some special steps are taken in educational therapy. They are ‘pure’<br />

in that the victims are free from mental defect, serious primary neurotic<br />

traits and all gross neurological deficits. This syndrome of developmental<br />

dyslexia is of constitutional and not of environmental origin and is<br />

often—perhaps even always—genetically determined. It is independent of<br />

the factor of intelligence and consequently may appear in children of<br />

normal IQ while standing out conspicuously in those who are in the above<br />

average brackets. The syndrome occurs more often in boys. The difficulty<br />

in learning to read is not due to peripheral visual anomalies but represents<br />

a higher level defect—an asymbolia. As an asymbolia, the problem in

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!