10.04.2013 Views

Linguistics Encyclopedia.pdf

Linguistics Encyclopedia.pdf

Linguistics Encyclopedia.pdf

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

The linguistics encyclopedia 336<br />

language will serve for the child, and the meanings are learnt during interaction with<br />

other people.<br />

Halliday’s study of Nigel’s language begins when Nigel is nine months old, a stage<br />

which most researchers would describe as prelinguistic. Halliday (1975, p. 14), however,<br />

takes a constant concomitance between sound and meaning, or expression and content, as<br />

qualification for a child sound to be part of a language, provided that it can be shown that<br />

this sound-expression pair ‘can be interpreted by reference to a set of prior established<br />

functions’ (ibid., p. 15). While adult language is usually thought to have three levels—<br />

sound, syntax, and meaning—the child language at this early stage is said by Halliday to<br />

have no syntax level: each element of the language is a content-expression pair (ibid., p.<br />

12). Furthermore, the expressions at this stage bear no necessary relation to the<br />

expressions of the adult language, although there is continuity between child and adult<br />

language in Halliday’s model, as we shall see below.<br />

The prior established functions with reference to which the child’s early utterances are<br />

interpreted are derived from Halliday’s functional theory of language (see<br />

FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR and FUNCTIONALIST LINGUISTICS) and from<br />

considerations of Bernstein’s notion of critical socializing contexts (see LANGUAGE<br />

AND EDUCATION). The functions, with ‘translations’, are (see Halliday, 1975, pp. 18–<br />

21):<br />

1 Instrumental; the I want function, by means of which the child satisfies its material<br />

needs.<br />

2 Regulatory; the do that function, by means of which the child regulates the behaviour<br />

of others.<br />

3 Interactional; the me and you function, by means of which the child interacts with<br />

others.<br />

4 Personal; the here I come function, by means of which the child expresses its own<br />

uniqueness.<br />

5 Heuristic; the tell me why function, through which the child learns about and explores<br />

the environment.<br />

6 Imaginative; the let’s pretend function, whereby the child creates an environment of its<br />

own.<br />

7 Informative; the I’ve got something to tell you function of language as a means of<br />

conveying information. This function appears much later than the others, in Nigel’s<br />

case at around twenty-two months of age.<br />

Within each function there is a range of options in meaning at each particular stage of the<br />

learning process, and this range increases within each function as the child’s language<br />

develops. At nine months old, Nigel had only two expressions which had constant<br />

meanings, one interactional, the other personal. But since there were no alternatives<br />

within each function, these expressions did not constitute a linguistic system. The first<br />

such system that Halliday accredits Nigel with derives from the time when he is 10½<br />

months old, when he employs the first four functions listed above, with alternatives in<br />

each. For instance, in the instrumental function Nigel has two options: a general demand,<br />

/nã/, which Halliday glosses as meaning ‘give me that’ and a more specific demand, /bø/,<br />

‘give me my toy bird’.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!