10.04.2013 Views

Linguistics Encyclopedia.pdf

Linguistics Encyclopedia.pdf

Linguistics Encyclopedia.pdf

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

A-Z 113<br />

polygenesis (multiple origin), and it found support in DeCamp’s (1971a, p. 24) argument<br />

that there are ‘certain pidgins and creoles which clearly developed without any direct<br />

Portuguese influence’. In fact, few creolists would argue for a pure monogenesis theory,<br />

but most accept that a certain amount of relexification is an important element in the<br />

development of pidgins and creoles, particularly when closely related lexicons, such as<br />

Creole Spanish and Creole Portuguese are involved (Holm, 1988, pp. 51–2).<br />

THE DEVELOPMENT OF PIDGINS AND<br />

CREOLES<br />

A particularly interesting and provocative explanation for the development and<br />

characteristics of creoles has been offered by Bickerton (1974, 1977, 1979, 1981, 1984a).<br />

Bickerton argues (1984, p. 173; emphasis added) ‘in favor of a language bioprogram<br />

hypothesis (henceforth LBH) that suggests that the infrastructure of language is<br />

specified at least as narrowly as Chomsky has claimed’. The arguments for LBH are<br />

drawn from Bickerton’s observations about the way in which a creole language develops<br />

from a pidgin which is in an early stage of development (ibid.):<br />

The LBH claims that the innovative aspects of creole grammar are<br />

inventions on the part of the first generation of children who have a pidgin<br />

as their linguistic input, rather than features transmitted from preexisting<br />

languages. The LBH claims, further, that such innovations show a degree<br />

of similarity, across wide variety in linguistic background, that is too great<br />

to be attributed to chance. Finally, the LBH claims that the most cogent<br />

explanation of this similarity is that it derives from the structure of a<br />

species-specific program for language, genetically coded and expressed,<br />

in ways still largely mysterious, in the structures and modes of operation<br />

of the human brain.<br />

The data Bickerton uses to support his hypothesis shows early-stage pidgin to lack any<br />

consistent means of marking tense, aspect, and modality, to have no consistent system of<br />

anaphora, no complex sentences, no systematic way of distinguishing case relations, and<br />

variable word order (1984a, p. 175). Children faced with this type of input impose ways<br />

of realizing the missing features, but they do not borrow these realizations from the<br />

language which is dominant in their environment, nor from the substrate language(s), and<br />

Bickerton concludes that ‘the LBH or some variant thereof seems inescapable…[and] the<br />

LBH carries profound implications for the study of language in general, and for the study<br />

of language acquisition and language origins in particular’ (1984a, p. 184).<br />

Bickerton claims (p. 178) that the evidence he cites shows the similarities in creoles to<br />

arise from ‘a single substantive grammar consisting of a very restricted set of categories<br />

and processes, which…constitute part, or all, of the human species-specific capacity for<br />

syntax’. He leans towards the view that the single, substantive grammar does, in fact,<br />

constitute all of universal grammar, and he thinks that this view is supported by Slobin’s<br />

(1977, 1982, 1984b) notion of a basic child grammar, a grammar which is generated by

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!