10.04.2013 Views

Linguistics Encyclopedia.pdf

Linguistics Encyclopedia.pdf

Linguistics Encyclopedia.pdf

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

of the range of application of the passive (Ingram, 1989, p. 471). According to Pinker<br />

(1984), children use their knowledge of oblique prepositional phrases such as The cat was<br />

sitting by the fence to generate what they first perceive as a parallel structure, e.g. The cat<br />

was bitten by the dog (Ingram, 1989, p. 472):<br />

This will eventually lead to a new phrase-structure rule which generates<br />

passives as well as oblique prepositional phrases. The difference between<br />

the two rules will be in the grammatical roles of the NPs. A last step will<br />

be the child’s acquiring a second lexical entry for the passive form of the<br />

verb. For example, the child’s lexicon will have ‘bite’ for active sentences<br />

and ‘bitten’ for passive ones, [compare LEXICAL-FUNCTIONAL<br />

GRAMMAR].<br />

Children under five years of age use few relative clauses, preferring to string clauses<br />

together with and (Ingram, 1989, p. 476). Yule (1985, p. 144) provides a typicalexample<br />

from a two-year-old who repeated an adult’s utterance The owl who eats candy runs fast<br />

as Owl eat candy and he run fast. As in the case of passives, it appears that four-year-old<br />

children have begun to acquire relative clauses, though they use them far less than adults<br />

do (Ingram, 1989, p. 483).<br />

C.Chomsky (1969) provides evidence of 5- to 10-year-old children’s abilities to<br />

process three types of constructions with pronominalization: (1) blocked backward<br />

pronominalization, as in He found out that Mickey won the race; (2) backwards<br />

pronominalization, as in Before he went out, Pluto took a nap; (3) forward<br />

pronominalization, as in Pluto thinks he knows everything. Each of the forty children in<br />

Chomsky’s study was tested for their comprehension of five sentences of each type. The<br />

children were first introduced to two dolls called Mickey and Pluto. Then the test<br />

sentence and a question about it were presented to the child, e.g. Pluto thinks that he<br />

knows everything. Pluto thinks that who knows everything? Because the children had two<br />

dolls in their universe of discourse, the pronoun in sentences of types 2 and 3 could refer<br />

to either the doll named in the sentence or to the other doll, so Chomsky only took<br />

account of the children’s performance with type 1 sentences. She took blocked backward<br />

pronominalization to be acquired by a child only if the child answered all five questions<br />

for this type of sentence correctly. Thirty-one of the children met this criterion, but all of<br />

them except three showed evidence of beginning acquisition. Chomsky concludes that the<br />

acquisition of pronominalization is maturationally determined, that is, independent of<br />

environmental factors and of intelligence and general cognitive development (Ingram,<br />

1989, p. 489).<br />

Later studies, for instance Ingram and Shaw (1981), in which a 100 children between<br />

three and eight years of age were studied, have shown four stages in the acquisition of<br />

pronominal reference, which show children to be moving from the exclusive use of linear<br />

order to a system which takes account of structural properties of sentences. The stages are<br />

(Ingram, 1989, p. 491):<br />

stage<br />

1:<br />

stage<br />

2:<br />

The linguistics encyclopedia 334<br />

Use of coreference: a pronoun may refer to an NP in a clause which may either precede or<br />

follow it;<br />

Use of linear order, a pronoun may only refer to a preceding NP;

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!