10.04.2013 Views

Linguistics Encyclopedia.pdf

Linguistics Encyclopedia.pdf

Linguistics Encyclopedia.pdf

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

A-Z 67<br />

The system can be made to cope with garden-path type ambiguity (see<br />

PSYCHOLINGUISTICS, pp. 370–1) by trying successive analyses of the problematic<br />

structure until one is found which the context allows. Thus, had the input sentence been<br />

The old train the young, the processor would have attempted the noun-phrase analysis of<br />

the old train first, but would have reached a dead end at state S1, when the current word is<br />

the while the arc demands that the current word be a verb. In such a situation, the<br />

processor must backtrack over the input and arcs taken previously trying alternative arcs<br />

at each state. In the case of The old train the young, when the backtracking process<br />

reaches state NP1 old can be recognized as a noun on arc 7, and the rest of the analysis<br />

follows straightforwardly (Wanner and Maratsos, 1978, p. 128). If such an approach is<br />

persistently adopted, a separate sequence of arcs is required for each sentence type that<br />

displays grammatical functions in different arrangements.<br />

The ATN can, however, be given the power to alter a function label on any given<br />

element if subsequent context demands it, so that when faced with a sentence in the<br />

passive voice, for example, it may alter the label SUBJECT, which it has given to the<br />

initial noun phrase, to OBJECT after it has recognized passive voice. This recognition<br />

can be accomplished by adding CAT V arcs, which test for the presence of be and a past<br />

participle ending on the main verb.<br />

Finally, an ATN can postpone a decision about the grammatical function of a<br />

problematic item by tagging it with HOLD. Elements in the HOLD list can be<br />

RETRIEVEd and assigned a function later in the analysis, when there is enough context<br />

to determine what the function should be (ibid. pp. 130–1). The HOLD list is particularly<br />

useful during the processing of relative clauses because it allows the ATN grammar to<br />

represent relative-clause patterns as systematic deformations of declarative-clause<br />

patterns. This strategy captures a grammatical generalization about the structural<br />

similarities between declarative and relative clauses (ibid., p. 137).<br />

Wanner and Maratsos (1978, pp. 132–7) show how the grammar outlined above can<br />

be extended to handle restrictive, unreduced, non-extraposed relative clauses, that is,<br />

clauses which immediately follow and modify a head noun by limiting the range of<br />

possible entities it can refer to (restrictive), which are introduced by a relative pronoun<br />

(unreduced) and which are structurally identical to independent declarative clauses<br />

(non-extraposed) except that one element is missing. Wanner and Maratsos (1978, p.<br />

132) give the following examples (head noun phrases in italics, the gap where an element<br />

is missing indicated by———):<br />

…the girl who———talked to the teacher about the problem…<br />

…the teacher whom the girl talked to———about the problem…<br />

…the problem that the girl talked to the teacher about———…<br />

As these examples demonstrate, the function fulfilled by the head noun is the same as the<br />

function which would have been fulfilled at the gap had the relative clause been an<br />

independent declarative clause:<br />

The girl talked to the teacher about the problem<br />

The girl talked to the teacher about the problem<br />

The girl talked to the teacher about the problem

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!