10.04.2013 Views

Linguistics Encyclopedia.pdf

Linguistics Encyclopedia.pdf

Linguistics Encyclopedia.pdf

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

A-Z 395<br />

a space between the parts of the compound (night owl, hammer and sickle); verb<br />

compounds (phrasal verbs like give up); non-verb compounds and idioms (at all, hammer<br />

and tongs, in front of); verb idioms (kick the bucket). In general, English-language<br />

dictionaries have a far higher proportion of main entries than dictionaries of other<br />

languages.<br />

One important class of possible subentries is derivatives whose meaning is that of the<br />

sum of their parts, such as lameness from lame and prewar from war. By convention,<br />

such derivatives, unlike nocturnal owl, are regarded as lexical units despite their<br />

semantic transparency, that is, in spite of the fact that their meaning is easily<br />

understood on the basis of the meanings of the parts of which they are composed. Large<br />

diction aries may make them main entries; many smaller dictionaries make them<br />

subentries to save space. However, such subentries are presented without explicit<br />

explanation of their meaning. Those formed by suffixation (lameness) are entered under<br />

their source (lame) as so-called undefined run-ons; those formed by prefixation (prewar)<br />

are in English-language dictionaries typically listed in alphabetical order under the<br />

prefix, e.g. pre-; but in some dictionaries of other languages, e.g. those, like the Larousse<br />

DFC and Lexis, that homograph by derivational families, they appear out of alphabetical<br />

order under their sources, with cross-references to them from their proper alphabetical<br />

position in the macrostructure.<br />

3 Graphically identical homologues (homographs, like l bank n, 2 bank n, 3 bank v) may<br />

be ordered historically—older before newer; by perceived frequency—more frequent<br />

before less frequent; or even by the alphabetical order of their part of speech—adjective<br />

before noun before verb. For graphically similar homologues, a variety of related<br />

algorithms may be used, such as lower-case before capital (creole, Creole), solid before<br />

spaced (rundown, run down), apostrophe before hyphen(s) (o’, -o-)—or any of these rules<br />

may be reversed!<br />

LEXICALLY RELEVANT INFORMATION<br />

Dictionaries provide any or all of the following types of lexically relevant information<br />

about the lexically relevant units they enter:<br />

1 Information about the etymology, or origin, of the unit.<br />

2 Information about the form of the unit, including spelling(s) and pronunciation(s).<br />

3 Syntactic categorization and subcategorization. In the first instance this information is<br />

given by a part-of-speech label (noun, verb etc.), but subcategorization can be supplied<br />

to any delicacy desired; that is, in finer and finer detail. Thus a lexical unit represented<br />

by the word-form tell may be categorized as verb, verb transitive (tell the truth), or<br />

verb ditransitive (tell them the truth).<br />

4 Inflections. Thus, the entry for tell will show that its past and past participle are told.<br />

5 Derivatives, especially if, like lameness, they are of the semantically transparent type<br />

that can qualify as undefined run-ons.<br />

6 ‘Paradigmatic’ information, such as synonyms (same meaning), antonyms (opposite<br />

meaning), superordinates (crippled is superordinate to one sense of lame), converses<br />

(like buy for sell), and even paronyms or confusibles (like imply for infer). A special

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!