10.04.2013 Views

Linguistics Encyclopedia.pdf

Linguistics Encyclopedia.pdf

Linguistics Encyclopedia.pdf

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

A-Z 333<br />

years old, and she showed that five- and six-year-old children were able to add the<br />

appropriate grammatical suffixes to invented words when the words’ grammatical class<br />

was clear from the context. Her experimental procedure has become known as the wug<br />

procedure, wug being one of the invented words used in the experiment.<br />

This experiment and others like it may be used to argue for the hypothesis that<br />

children are ‘tuned in’, not only to the sounds of human language (see above) but also to<br />

its syntax, in the sense that they display ‘a strong tendency…to analyse the formal<br />

aspects of the linguistic input’ (Karmiloff-Smith, 1987, p. 369). Karmiloff-Smith (1979)<br />

shows that French children determine gender by attending to word endings from about<br />

the age of three, and Levy (1983) produces similar findings for Hebrew-speaking<br />

children. Karmiloff-Smith (1987, p. 370) argues that, since a child will get its message<br />

through without all the correct syntactic markers attached to it, the child must be showing<br />

analytical interest in adding them when it does so.<br />

The order of acquisition of grammatical morphemes in English tends to be that -ing<br />

appears first, then the regular plural -s, then possessive -s and irregular past-tense forms,<br />

before the regular forms (Yule, 1985, pp. 143–4) (see the discussion of the generalization<br />

principle above). Yule (1985, pp. 144–5) isolates three stages for the acquisition of the<br />

most studied syntactic categories: question and negative formations. Stage I occurs<br />

between 18 and 26 months; stage II between 22 and 30 months; and stage III between 24<br />

and 40 months. During stage I, children either simply begin any question with a wh- form<br />

such as where or who, or they use rising intonation. To form negatives, children at this<br />

stage simply begin the utterance with no or not. During stage II of question formation,<br />

intonation is still used, but more wh- forms become available. For negative formation,<br />

don’t and can’t begin to appear, and both these forms and no and not are placed in front<br />

of the verb instead of at the beginning of the utterance. In stage III, questions have the<br />

required inversion of subject and verb, although wh- forms do not always undergo the<br />

inversion: Can I have a piece versus Why kitty can’t stand up? Didn’t and won’t appear<br />

for negatives during stage III, with isn’t appearing very late (see Ingram, 1989, Ch. 9, for<br />

a far more detailed account, including reports of several studies of Subject-Aux<br />

Inversion).<br />

Ingram (1989) discusses a number of proposed explanations for this acquisition<br />

pattern. According to performance-factor approaches, children have the adult rules<br />

available to them from the start, but are prevented by performance factors, such as<br />

limitations of memory, from applying them. According to competence-factor<br />

approaches, the child’s speech reflects its grammar at the time, and the adult rules are<br />

acquired gradually, restricted at first to specific contexts and then increasingly<br />

generalized. Thus Kuczaj and Brannick (1979) propose that the child first learns to invert<br />

subject and verb for certain specific wh- words, and then gradually learns to do it for all<br />

wh- words.<br />

Other areas of syntax which have received much attention include passives, relative<br />

clauses, and the use of pronouns to refer to noun phrases in the surrounding text.<br />

Horgan (1978) shows that whereas children aged between two and four years<br />

recognize the passive form, they tend to misunderstand the relationship between the<br />

participants described by reversible passives such as The cat was chased by the girl;<br />

children tend to use this form to describe a picture of a cat chasing a girl. Horgan’s and<br />

other studies suggest that it takes several years for children to acquire full understanding

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!