30.11.2012 Views

Automotive User Interfaces and Interactive Vehicular Applications

Automotive User Interfaces and Interactive Vehicular Applications

Automotive User Interfaces and Interactive Vehicular Applications

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

incorporate natural breakpoints will only influence performance<br />

when users set appropriate priorities (i.e., “safety first”).<br />

A second problem is to choose the right amount of breakpoints. In<br />

a study where multiple breakpoints were provided we again found<br />

that people use these breakpoints [12]. However, they did not use<br />

all of them. This was probably because additional interleaving did<br />

not improve performance strongly. Future work should point out<br />

to what extent people can be guided in other ways (for example<br />

using feedback [13]) in how they make performance trade-offs.<br />

3. GENERAL DISCUSSION<br />

In this paper we have taken the position that considering natural<br />

breakpoints when designing in-car devices can be beneficial.<br />

Interleaving at natural breakpoints offers users many advantages,<br />

<strong>and</strong> people show a tendency to interleave here. Good design might<br />

therefore encourage how frequent people interleave between tasks<br />

by providing sufficient breakpoints. In the context of in-car<br />

systems this might make the difference between a driver that<br />

checks the road frequently <strong>and</strong> one that ignores the road<br />

altogether. However, there are limitations to this approach. Most<br />

importantly, drivers who don’t set safety as their priority tend to<br />

ignore cues for interleaving. It is therefore important to make<br />

them aware of the impact of multitasking on safety [see also 19].<br />

A limitation of our studies is that they were conducted in a lab<br />

setting. Surely in the real-world people’s priority is always to<br />

drive safely? Unfortunately this doesn’t seem to be the case.<br />

Closed test track studies have shown that people multitask in the<br />

car, even when they have full knowledge about future safer (nonmultitask)<br />

opportunities to perform a secondary task [8].<br />

Similarly, in-car phone use is still observed frequently [18].<br />

A complementary design solution to our work is to reduce drivers’<br />

need to make performance trade-offs altogether. For example,<br />

phones that have speech interfaces reduce the need to share visual<br />

<strong>and</strong> manual resources between the phone <strong>and</strong> the road. However,<br />

this approach also has limitations. First, the reduction of visualmanual<br />

interaction does not necessarily make it safe to make a<br />

phone call while driving (e.g., [10]). Second, even when “safer”<br />

interfaces are available, users might not always choose to use<br />

them, if the interaction style does not serve their objective. For<br />

example, audio interfaces might reduce visual distraction, but can<br />

also be slow to use compared to visual interfaces. <strong>User</strong>s might<br />

therefore choose to use a visual interface over an audio interface if<br />

they want to complete a task quickly [5]. Due to these limitations<br />

it is still worthwhile to think about natural breakpoints in tasks.<br />

4. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS<br />

This work was sponsored by EPSRC grant EP ⁄ G043507 ⁄ 1.<br />

5. REFERENCES<br />

[1] Altmann, E.M. <strong>and</strong> Trafton, J.G., Memory for goals: An<br />

activation-based model. Cognitive Sci, 26,1 (2002), 39-83.<br />

[2] Bailey, B.P. <strong>and</strong> Iqbal, S.T., Underst<strong>and</strong>ing changes in<br />

mental workload during execution of goal-directed tasks <strong>and</strong><br />

its application for interruption management. ACM T Comput-<br />

Hum Int, 14,4 (2008), 1-28.<br />

[3] Borst, J.P., Taatgen, N.A., <strong>and</strong> Van Rijn, H., The Problem<br />

State: A Cognitive Bottleneck in Multitasking. J Exp Psychol<br />

Learn, 36,2 (2010), 363-382.<br />

[4] Brumby, D.P., Salvucci, D.D., <strong>and</strong> Howes, A., Focus on<br />

Driving: How Cognitive Constraints Shape the Adaptation of<br />

Strategy when Dialing while Driving. In SIGCHI Conference<br />

on Human Factors in Computing Systems, ACM Press<br />

(2009), 1629-1638.<br />

[5] Brumby, D.P., Davies, S., Janssen, C.P., <strong>and</strong> Grace, J.J., Fast<br />

or Safe? How Performance Objectives Determine Modality<br />

Output Choices while Interacting on the Move In SIGCHI<br />

Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, ACM<br />

Press (2011), 473-482.<br />

[6] Card, S.K., Moran, T.P., <strong>and</strong> Newell, A., The psychology of<br />

human-computer interaction. Lawrence Erlbaum (1983).<br />

[7] Diels, C., Reed, N., <strong>and</strong> Weaver, L., Drivers’ attitudes to<br />

distraction <strong>and</strong> other motorists’ behaviour: a focus group <strong>and</strong><br />

observational study. TRL Limited, Wokingham, UK, 2009.<br />

[8] Horrey, W.J. <strong>and</strong> Lesch, M.F., Driver-Initiated distractions:<br />

Examining strategic adaptation for in-vehicle task initiation.<br />

Accid Anal Prev, 41,(2009), 115-122.<br />

[9] Iqbal, S.T. <strong>and</strong> Bailey, B.P., Oasis: A framework for linking<br />

notification delivery to the perceptual structure of goaldirected<br />

tasks. ACM T Comput-Hum Int, 17,4 (2010), 15:1-<br />

28.<br />

[10] Iqbal, S.T., Ju, Y.-C., <strong>and</strong> Horvitz, E., Cars, Calls, <strong>and</strong><br />

Cognition: Investigating Driving <strong>and</strong> Divided Attention. In<br />

SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing<br />

Systems, ACM Press (2010), 1281-1290.<br />

[11] Janssen, C.P. <strong>and</strong> Brumby, D.P., Strategic adaptation to<br />

performance objectives in a dual-task setting. Cognitive Sci,<br />

34,8 (2010), 1548-1560.<br />

[12] Janssen, C.P., Brumby, D.P., <strong>and</strong> Garnett, R., Natural Break<br />

Points: The Influence of Priorities, <strong>and</strong> Cognitive <strong>and</strong> Motor<br />

Cues on Dual-Task Interleaving. Journal of Cognitive<br />

Engineering <strong>and</strong> Decision Making, (in press).<br />

[13] Janssen, C.P., Brumby, D.P., Dowell, J., Chater, N., <strong>and</strong><br />

Howes, A., Identifying Optimum Performance Trade-Offs<br />

using a Cognitively Bounded Rational Analysis Model of<br />

Discretionary Task Interleaving. Topics in Cognitive Science,<br />

3,1 (2011), 123-139.<br />

[14] Navon, D. <strong>and</strong> Gopher, D., On the Economy of the Human-<br />

Processing System. Psychol Rev, 86,3 (1979), 214-255.<br />

[15] Norman, D.A. <strong>and</strong> Bobrow, D.G., On Data-limited <strong>and</strong><br />

Resource-limited Processes. Cognitive Psychol, 7,1 (1975),<br />

44-64.<br />

[16] Payne, S.J., Duggan, G.B., <strong>and</strong> Neth, H., Discretionary Task<br />

Interleaving: Heuristics for Time Allocation in Cognitive<br />

Foraging. J Exp Psychol Gen, 136,3 (2007), 370-388.<br />

[17] Salvucci, D.D. <strong>and</strong> Taatgen, N.A., The Multitasking Mind.<br />

Oxford University Press (2011).<br />

[18] The Economist, "Think before you speak: Distracted driving<br />

is the new drunk driving", 2011.<br />

[19] Wang, Y., Zhang, W., Reimer, B., Lavallière, M., Lesch,<br />

M.F., Horrey, W.J., <strong>and</strong> Wu, S., The Effect of Feedback on<br />

Attitudes Toward Cellular Phone Use While Driving: A<br />

Comparison Between Novice <strong>and</strong> Experienced Drivers.<br />

Traffic Injury Prevention, 11,5 (2010), 471-477.<br />

[20] Wickens, C.D., Multiple Resources <strong>and</strong> Mental Workload.<br />

Hum Factors, 50,3 (2008), 449-455.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!