14.12.2012 Views

The Context of HIV Risk Among Drug Users and Their Sexual Partners

The Context of HIV Risk Among Drug Users and Their Sexual Partners

The Context of HIV Risk Among Drug Users and Their Sexual Partners

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

aggravate self-presentation bias. This evidence, which was culled from a<br />

large number <strong>of</strong> studies, also suggested that task variables (e.g., the<br />

structure <strong>and</strong> length <strong>of</strong> questions <strong>and</strong> how the interview was<br />

administered) were important factors influencing response, with memory<br />

variables <strong>and</strong> the characteristics <strong>of</strong> interviewers <strong>and</strong> respondents being<br />

much less important.<br />

In a later study, Bradburn <strong>and</strong> Sudman (1979) attempted to determine<br />

whether more anonymous forms <strong>of</strong> data collection (self-administered<br />

questionnaire or telephone survey) might reduce response bias relative to<br />

that which occurs when interviews are conducted face-to-face. <strong>The</strong><br />

questions included highly undesirable behaviors such as drunken driving<br />

<strong>and</strong> bankruptcy. While the more anonymous interview did not<br />

consistently increase levels <strong>of</strong> behavior reported, variations in question<br />

structure <strong>and</strong> length <strong>and</strong> structure did. Subjects were r<strong>and</strong>omized to<br />

closed-ended versus open-ended questions, long versus short questions,<br />

<strong>and</strong> questions with st<strong>and</strong>ard versus familiar wording. Familiarly worded<br />

questions are questions that employ language that respondents themselves<br />

would use. <strong>The</strong> results indicated that long, open-ended, familiarly<br />

worded questions yielded higher levels <strong>of</strong> reporting than short, closedended<br />

questions with st<strong>and</strong>ard wording. <strong>The</strong> pattern was evident even<br />

when differences in formats were examined on an item-by-item basis.<br />

Very few interactions <strong>of</strong> formats were noted.<br />

Impact <strong>of</strong> Questionnaire Format<br />

In the present study, the authors examined the impact <strong>of</strong> question length<br />

<strong>and</strong> question order on the extent <strong>of</strong> underreporting <strong>and</strong> zero-reporting <strong>of</strong><br />

high-risk practices. Without a gold st<strong>and</strong>ard against which to validate<br />

self-reports, the authors made the assumption that higher levels <strong>of</strong><br />

reported risk behaviors would on average be more accurate since selfreports<br />

are downwardly biased when respondents find questions to be<br />

threatening or to invite socially desirable responses (Catania et al. 1990).<br />

<strong>The</strong> authors hypothesized that long questions <strong>and</strong> questions asked late<br />

rather than early in the interview would result in higher levels <strong>of</strong><br />

reporting. <strong>The</strong> long questions (15 words or more) were preceded by<br />

preambles that essentially encouraged respondents to acknowledge<br />

socially undesirable (risk) behavior. For example, the question about<br />

needle sharing was prefaced with the statement, “Now we have some<br />

questions about needles <strong>and</strong> syringes. <strong>The</strong>re are reasons people share<br />

used outfits, such as being dope sick, not having your own works, <strong>and</strong> not<br />

230

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!