09.04.2013 Views

2012 COURSE DATES: AUGUST 4 – 17, 2012 - Sirenian International

2012 COURSE DATES: AUGUST 4 – 17, 2012 - Sirenian International

2012 COURSE DATES: AUGUST 4 – 17, 2012 - Sirenian International

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

MANN: BEHAVIORAL SAMPLING METHODS 103<br />

tacean behavior have received less attention. In this paper, I review quantitative<br />

observational methods typically used in cetacean studies and offer specific sam-<br />

pling suggestions.<br />

Altmann (1974) points out that most field observations of behavior involve<br />

sampling decisions, whether those decisions are explicit or not.<br />

“Sampling decisions are made wbenever the stua’ent of social behavior cannot continuously<br />

observe and record all of the behavior of all of the members of a social group. . , . We<br />

suspect that the investigator ofen chooses a sampling procedure without being aware that<br />

he is making a choice. Of course, he does not thereby escape fiom the consequences of that<br />

choice.” (Altmann 1974:229).<br />

The lack of an explicit protocol for sampling was termed ad libitum sam-<br />

pling by Altmann (1974), and she pointed out that it typically entails scoring<br />

“as much as one can” or whatever is most readily observable of the behavior<br />

of a group (Altmann 1974:235-236). These kinds of observations may be<br />

necessary as one learns to identify behaviors, as one plans a study, or as one<br />

observes rare but significant events. However, ad libitum observations suffer<br />

from a variety of potential biases. Different individuals may be more or less<br />

visible. Some behaviors may be more salient and more readily recorded than<br />

others. Individual animals may alter their behavior depending on how visible<br />

they are to other animals (and to observers). The same observer may concen-<br />

trate on different behaviors during different observation periods, and different<br />

observers may notice or attend to entirely different behaviors during the same<br />

observation period. Such biases indicate that ad libitum data are not appropriate<br />

for estimating rates of behaviors or for comparing rates across subjects or across<br />

studies. The selection and appropriate use of sampling methods that yield<br />

unbiased estimates of behavior are critical to the scientific validity of any study.<br />

In this paper, first I review the methods employed in the majority of recent<br />

cetacean field studies, including sampling method and protocols, and discuss<br />

the general advantages and pitfalls of each sampling method. Second, I rec-<br />

ommend specific sampling methods for cetaceans, depending on identifiability<br />

of animals, group sizes, dive durations, and change in group membership.<br />

Literature Survey<br />

To evaluate what methods are currently used by cetologists, I surveyed<br />

papers on cetacean behavior published from 1989 through 1995 (see Appendix<br />

1 for details). To limit the survey, I selected two peer-reviewed journals that<br />

together published the majority of studies in wild cetacean behavior, The Ca-<br />

nadian Journal of Zoology (CJZ, 31% of studies) and Marine Mammal Science<br />

(MMS, 29% of studies).<br />

Seventy-four studies were reviewed, 38 in CJZ and 36 in MMS. The fol-<br />

lowing information was noted: species, age and sex classes, number of animals,<br />

number of observation or recording hours, group-size minima and maxima,<br />

whether animals were individually identified, types of behaviors recorded (in-<br />

cluding vocalizations), definition of group and definition of behaviors, and

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!