06.01.2013 Views

Physical fitness training for stroke patients (Review) - Update Software

Physical fitness training for stroke patients (Review) - Update Software

Physical fitness training for stroke patients (Review) - Update Software

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Compliance with the scheduled exercise programme during <strong>training</strong><br />

sessions was described in only six trials. For cardiorespiratory<br />

<strong>training</strong> interventions, Langhammer 2007 stated that the compliance<br />

with the individualised <strong>training</strong> levels was ’high’, Pohl 2002<br />

reported ’excellent tolerance’ of treadmill <strong>training</strong>, and Salbach<br />

2004 maintained that most of the participants completed nine<br />

out of 10 circuit <strong>training</strong> exercises. For mixed <strong>training</strong>, Duncan<br />

1998 reported ’good compliance’ with home-based <strong>training</strong> and<br />

Yang 2006 stated that mixed circuit <strong>training</strong> was ’per<strong>for</strong>med as<br />

planned’. Mead 2007 reported 94% to 99% compliance with circuit<br />

<strong>training</strong> exercises ’tailored’ to individual requirements. In<strong>for</strong>mation<br />

on compliance was not available <strong>for</strong> the remaining trials.<br />

Risk of bias in included studies<br />

Randomisation<br />

One trial adopted a cross-over design with random allocation to the<br />

order of the treatment sequences (Moore 2010). For the purpose of<br />

this review we only analysed the results at the end of the first period,<br />

as deriving from parallel groups. All remaining trials adopted a<br />

parallel group design.<br />

Methods of sequence generation were clearly reported in 16 out<br />

of the 32 included trials. These included methods such as drawing<br />

lots (Bale 2008), throwing dice (Langhammer 2007; Smith<br />

2008), picking envelopes (Eich 2004; Pohl 2002; Yang 2006),<br />

random number tables (da Cunha 2002), or computer-generated<br />

lists (Bateman 2001; Cooke 2010; Donaldson 2009; James 2002;<br />

Lennon 2008; Mead 2007; Mudge 2009; Salbach 2004; Sims<br />

2009). To balance the numbers of participants to be assigned to<br />

each intervention group block randomisation was used in 14 trials<br />

(Bateman 2001; Cooke 2010; Donaldson 2009; Duncan 1998;<br />

Duncan 2003; James 2002; Katz-Leurer 2003; Lennon 2008; Pohl<br />

2002; Richards 1993; Richards 2004; Salbach 2004; Sims 2009;<br />

Teixeira 1999). To balance participant characteristics between intervention<br />

groups stratified randomisation was used in 11 trials.<br />

Allocation to intervention groups was stratified according to different<br />

participants’ characteristics: by gait per<strong>for</strong>mance (Moore<br />

2010; Pohl 2002; Salbach 2004); by gender (Sims 2009); by age<br />

and gender (Lennon 2008); by age, gender, and time since <strong>stroke</strong><br />

(Kim 2001); by age, gender, and disability (Mead 2007); by gender<br />

and hemispheric lesion (Langhammer 2007); by functional<br />

ability (Donaldson 2009; Richards 1993); and by <strong>stroke</strong> severity<br />

(Winstein 2004). Six trials were described as randomised but did<br />

not provide in<strong>for</strong>mation on the methods used <strong>for</strong> generating random<br />

assignments (Aidar 2007; Cuviello-Palmer 1988; Flansbjer<br />

2008; Glasser 1986; Inaba 1973; Ouellette 2004; Potempa 1995).<br />

Allocation concealment<br />

In<strong>for</strong>mation on allocation concealment was available in less then<br />

half of the included trials (13/32). One trial reported the use<br />

of a central assignment (Mead 2007), another trial the use of<br />

a third party (Duncan 1998), and two trials the use of sequentially<br />

numbered sealed opaque envelopes (Cooke 2010; Donaldson<br />

2009) as adequate mechanisms of allocation concealment. Most<br />

trials reported the use of ’sealed envelopes’ but did not specify<br />

whether they were sequentially numbered or opaque (Bateman<br />

2001; Duncan 2003; Eich 2004; James 2002; Lennon 2008;<br />

Moore 2010; Winstein 2004; Yang 2006), there<strong>for</strong>e we were unable<br />

to exclude potential selection bias with certainty.<br />

Intention-to-treat (ITT) analysis<br />

Fourteen trials reported the use of an ITT approach <strong>for</strong> their analyses<br />

(Bateman 2001; Donaldson 2009; Duncan 1998; Duncan<br />

2003; Eich 2004; Flansbjer 2008; James 2002; Langhammer<br />

2007; Mead 2007; Mudge 2009; Ouellette 2004; Potempa 1995;<br />

Richards 2004; Sims 2009) although one of these trials (Bateman<br />

2001) did not analyse data <strong>for</strong> the participants who dropped out<br />

(where possible, we imputed these missing data).<br />

Amongst the 18 trials that did not mention ITT, nine did not have<br />

any missing data (Bale 2008; Cuviello-Palmer 1988; Glasser 1986;<br />

Kim 2001; Moore 2010; Potempa 1995; Smith 2008; Teixeira<br />

1999; Yang 2006).<br />

Blinding<br />

Participant blinding<br />

Participants could not be blinded to physical <strong>training</strong>. In two trials,<br />

however, participants were in<strong>for</strong>med that they would receive one<br />

of two different, potentially beneficial interventions (Kim 2001;<br />

Mead 2007) without being given in<strong>for</strong>mation on the types of interventions.<br />

Similarly, in another trial (Donaldson 2009) participants<br />

allocated to the experimental group were advised that they<br />

were to be offered extra therapy but were not told which type of<br />

therapy.<br />

Investigator blinding<br />

<strong>Physical</strong> <strong>fitness</strong> <strong>training</strong> <strong>for</strong> <strong>stroke</strong> <strong>patients</strong> (<strong>Review</strong>)<br />

Copyright © 2011 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.<br />

The outcome assessors were reported to be blinded in 19 of<br />

the 32 included trials (Bale 2008; Bateman 2001; Cooke 2010;<br />

Donaldson 2009; Duncan 2003; Eich 2004; Flansbjer 2008;<br />

James 2002; Katz-Leurer 2003; Kim 2001; Langhammer 2007;<br />

Mead 2007; Mudge 2009; Ouellette 2004; Pohl 2002; Richards<br />

1993; Richards 2004; Salbach 2004; Yang 2006). Some of these<br />

trials reported, however, that some degree of unmasking might<br />

have occurred (Eich 2004; Mudge 2009; Salbach 2004). Participants<br />

were instructed not to reveal group assignments in four trials<br />

(Bateman 2001; Duncan 2003; Flansbjer 2008; Mead 2007).<br />

Outcome assessment was not blinded in three trials (Moore 2010;<br />

Smith 2008; Winstein 2004). Details of blinding were not provided<br />

in the remaining trials.<br />

10

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!