08.11.2014 Views

Mental health policy and practice across Europe: an overview

Mental health policy and practice across Europe: an overview

Mental health policy and practice across Europe: an overview

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Tackling social exclusion 51<br />

not homogenized into a common, global underst<strong><strong>an</strong>d</strong>ing of mental ill <strong>health</strong> as<br />

‘disease’.<br />

Several authors have discussed possible reasons for the frequent use of the<br />

brain illness/disease model in <strong>an</strong>ti-discriminatory work. Rose (1998) notes that<br />

in the 1950s <strong><strong>an</strong>d</strong> 1960s the notion of ‘<strong>an</strong> illness like <strong>an</strong>y other’ was central to the<br />

community psychiatry movement, which sought to reduce coercion in favour<br />

of care, strip madness of its terrors <strong><strong>an</strong>d</strong> make mental <strong>health</strong> services a br<strong>an</strong>ch of<br />

ordinary medicine. There is thus profound professional investment in the<br />

‘illness like <strong>an</strong>y other’ model, which may help explain its assumed usefulness<br />

in the absence of empirical evidence. Pharmaceutical comp<strong>an</strong>ies may have<br />

vested interests in the illness conception, as it opens up markets for the sale of<br />

bio-medical solutions. Relatives of mental <strong>health</strong> service users c<strong>an</strong> find comfort<br />

in the ‘brain disease’ model as the emphasis on nature rather th<strong>an</strong> nurture<br />

absolves them of behavioural responsibility. However, there is no evidence<br />

that <strong>an</strong>y particular model of what madness/mental illness is, or what causes it, is<br />

<strong>an</strong>y more useful th<strong>an</strong> <strong>an</strong>y other model in <strong>an</strong>ti-discrimination work. Far too<br />

much confidence has been placed in the brain disease model, which may compound<br />

rather th<strong>an</strong> challenge the stereotypes of d<strong>an</strong>gerousness <strong><strong>an</strong>d</strong>, particularly,<br />

incompetence.<br />

The assumption that public underst<strong><strong>an</strong>d</strong>ing of the ‘illness’ model is positive is<br />

so deep-rooted that it sometimes emerges in research studies as <strong>an</strong> assumed (not<br />

demonstrated) positive outcome. For inst<strong>an</strong>ce, the Royal College of Psychiatrists<br />

cited as a success of their Defeat Depression campaign <strong>an</strong> increase in the proportion<br />

of the public believing that depression was ‘a medical condition like <strong>an</strong>y<br />

other illnesses’ (Royal College of Psychiatrists 1995). Stuart <strong><strong>an</strong>d</strong> Arboleda Florez<br />

(2001) use knowledge that schizophrenia is a biological illness as one of several<br />

indicators of accurate public knowledge, in order to assess links between accurate<br />

knowledge <strong><strong>an</strong>d</strong> stigma. This type of research does not enable us to ascertain<br />

whether belief in <strong>an</strong> illness model is linked, positively or negatively, with<br />

import<strong>an</strong>t outcomes from the service user’s point of view, such as social dist<strong>an</strong>ce.<br />

It is necessary both for research <strong><strong>an</strong>d</strong> for evidence-based <strong>practice</strong> to<br />

discard the assumption that biological underst<strong><strong>an</strong>d</strong>ings reduce discrimination.<br />

Causation is not the key to countering discrimination.<br />

More promising are messages that ignore causation, focusing rather on contribution<br />

<strong><strong>an</strong>d</strong> equal citizenship, breaking the links with violence <strong><strong>an</strong>d</strong> incompetence.<br />

The disability rights model – which draws attention to disabling barriers<br />

<strong><strong>an</strong>d</strong> to people’s rights to participate, with adjustments where needed – seems<br />

much more likely to effect ch<strong>an</strong>ges in attitude <strong><strong>an</strong>d</strong> behaviour th<strong>an</strong> the chimera<br />

that as scientific underst<strong><strong>an</strong>d</strong>ing of causation percolates through society, so discrimination<br />

will ebb away. Evidence to support or contradict this approach is<br />

limited. However, the New Zeal<strong><strong>an</strong>d</strong> government’s Like Minds project appears to<br />

have had some success in reducing discriminatory attitudes, especially desire for<br />

social dist<strong>an</strong>ce, while there is some evidence that conventional programmes<br />

raising awareness of biological causation may not effectively reduce stigma<br />

(Angermeyer <strong><strong>an</strong>d</strong> Matschinger 2005). More research is needed in this area<br />

to provide <strong>policy</strong>-makers with evidence for how best to reduce discriminatory<br />

attitudes <strong><strong>an</strong>d</strong> behaviours.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!