12.12.2022 Views

BazermanMoore

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Positive Illusions 93

individual’s positive sense of self (Taylor & Brown, 1988, 1994). In addition, Taylor

and Brown argue that positive illusions increase personal commitment, help individuals

persist at difficult tasks, and facilitate coping with aversive and uncontrollable

events.

Certainly, it is reasonable to argue that positive illusions help to create entrepreneurs

who are willing to discount risks. Positive illusions allow us to maintain cognitive

consistency, belief in a just world, and perceived control (Greenwald, 1980). Seligman

(1991) advocates the selection of salespeople based on the magnitude of their positive

illusion—what he calls ‘‘learned optimism.’’ He argues that unrealistically high levels of

optimism bolster sales-force persistence.

We believe that each of these findings is true and that in some specific situations

(e.g., severe health conditions), positive illusions may prove beneficial. In addition, positive

illusions may help people cope with tragic events, particularly when they have few

alternatives and are not facing any major decisions. However, we also believe that this

adaptive story is incomplete—and, indeed, dangerous in most decision-making environments.

Every day, people invest their life savings in new businesses that have little

chance of success. Similarly, falsely assuming that they are irreplaceable, people make

ultimatums to their employers and end up losing their jobs.

Much of the evidence suggesting that positive illusions are adaptive shares a serious

problem: the studies do not measure illusions in predictions of future outcomes,

but instead pick up on accuracy in self-assessment (Klein & Steers-Wentzell, 2005).

These studies measure people’s belief that they are likely to be better off than others

(with respect to the severity of their cancer, their chances of career success, and so on)

and then go on to show that those who believe they are better off do in fact wind up

being better off. The simple explanation for these results is that people have an accurate

sense of how bad their cancer is or what their career prospects are. Indeed, evidence

suggests that high self-esteem is more likely to be a result of prior success rather

than a cause of it (Baumeister, Campbell, Krueger, & Vohs, 2003).

By contrast, positive illusions are hazardous (and even self-negating) when they

cause people to temporarily fool themselves into believing that they are better than

they are. In a study of college students, Robins and Beer (2001) found that positive

illusions are associated with higher levels of self-reported well-being and self-esteem in

the short term. But, over time, individuals become frustrated as they encounter evidence

that their academic performance is not as good as they think (see also McGraw,

Mellers, & Ritov, 2004). Positive illusions can lead people to behave in ways that are

arrogant, careless, and self-centered (Anderson, Srivastava, Beer, Spataro, & Chatman,

2006; Baumeister, Campbell, Krueger, & Vohs, 2003). Positive illusions can actually

undermine effort and performance (Stone, 1994). Those who engage in the most selfserving

reasoning are also more likely to engage in cheating, in part because they are

better at justifying the behavior to themselves (von Hippel, Lakin, & Shakarchi, 2005).

Our cynicism about the adaptive role of positive illusions is shared by a number of

scholars who caution that positive illusions are likely to have a negative impact on learning

and on the quality of decision making, personnel decisions, and responses to crises

(e.g., ‘‘global warming isn’t that bad’’), and can contribute to conflict and discontent

(Brodt, 1990; Dunning, Heath, & Suls, 2004; Kramer, 1994; Tyler & Hastie, 1991).

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!