12.12.2022 Views

BazermanMoore

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

feedback on the decision not chosen is inevitable, they can make choices that are likely

to compare favorably to foregone options. In this context, it may be useful to have your

‘‘should’’ self compare the various outcomes. The two selves also will need to negotiate

with each other over the relative value of better outcomes (such as winning a silver

rather than a bronze medal) compared with the pain of regret.

SUMMARY

Summary 99

Too often, people view their emotions as uncontrollable. The fact is, even if we can’t

stop ourselves from feeling, we may be able to limit the negative effects of our emotions

on the quality of our decisions. Johnson and Tversky (1983) and Lerner, Goldberg, and

Tetlock (1998) note that people are typically unaware of the influence of their emotions

on their decisions. Thus, though we may feel that we are angry, we may falsely believe

that anger will not influence our judgment. Perhaps a better appreciation of the literature

can help create the knowledge that ‘‘Just like everyone else, I am affected by my

emotional state.’’ It may simply help to be more aware of the ways in which emotion can

bias our judgments.

To neutralize the negative impact of our emotions on our decisions, we must begin

by identifying our emotions and their sources. Labeling our emotions in itself can be an

effective means of reducing their strength (Lieberman, Eisenberger, Crockett, Tom,

Pfeifer, & Way, 2007). It is well known among emotion researchers, for instance, that

asking research participants to explicitly identify their emotional state can often eliminate

the effect of an emotional manipulation. But unpleasant emotions are sometimes

even more effectively neutralized by identifying the source of the emotion because

doing so allows people to react to the causal stimulus with System 2’s more cognitive

assessments, rather than with System 1’s impulsive and emotional reactions. Earlier we

noted that the weather influences people’s reports of life satisfaction. But consider what

happens when, just before asking about a question about life satisfaction, a pollster asks

a respondent, ‘‘So how’s the weather where you are?’’ This question eliminates the effect

of weather on responses to the life-satisfaction question (Schwarz & Strack, 1999).

Another strategy for managing the negative impact of emotions is to make decision

makers accountable for their choices. Lerner and Tetlock (1999) have found that study

participants who must in some way justify their decisions learn to hold their emotions in

check and move toward more systematic, System 2 thinking. This may be because such

articulation is itself a System 2 process or because verbalization can neutralize an emotion.

To create such accountability for yourself, you might report your rationale for a

decision to your boss or simply write down for yourself an explanation for your decision.

Logically and empirically, the simple cognition of accountability has the ability to reduce

the likelihood of acting on emotions in ways that you will later regret.

Finally, it may be possible to institutionalize controls on emotion. It is widely

known that government policies can be overly influenced by the vividness of various

issues (see Chapter 2). As a result, we as a society tend to allocate scarce resources to

vivid concerns rather than to the issues for which scarce resources would do the most

good. Why? Vivid stories create emotional reactions; these emotions, in turn, lead us to

misallocate scarce resources. Sunstein (2002) argues, ‘‘Just as the Senate was designed

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!