12.12.2022 Views

BazermanMoore

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Focalism and the Focusing Illusion 49

likely to occur concurrently (Wilson, Wheatley, Meyers, Gilbert, & Axsom, 2000). As a

consequence, people tend to overestimate both the degree to which their future

thoughts will be occupied by the focal event and the duration of their emotional response

to the event. For example, we tend to overestimate the impact of positive and

negative events, such as the wins and losses of our preferred sports team or political

candidate, on our overall happiness. We even dramatically overestimate the effects on

our happiness of being afflicted by a major medical condition.

In a similar vein, Schkade and Kahneman (1998) define the focusing illusion as the

tendency of people to make judgments based on their attention to only a subset of

available information, to overweight that information, and to underweight unattended

information. Using logic similar to that of Gilbert, Wilson, and colleagues, Schkade and

Kahneman (1998) asked college students in the Midwest and in Southern California

about their own life satisfaction and the perceived life satisfaction of others. Californians

and Midwesterners reported a similar level of life satisfaction, yet both groups

rated Californians as having greater life satisfaction than Midwesterners. Essentially,

differences between California and the Midwest, such as climate, strongly influenced

nonresidents’ judgments of residents’ life satisfaction. However, these factors did not

predict the experienced life satisfaction of citizens of the two locales. Schkade and

Kahneman argue that when students imagined how a move to the other location would

affect them, the obvious difference of weather became a salient factor, and all other life

events affecting satisfaction were out of focus.

Imagine that eight teams in any game or sport are engaged in a single elimination

tournament. Now imagine that eight people are each assigned to each team and asked

the probability that ‘‘their’’ team will win the tournament. Of course, some teams would

be better, and some would be worse, but the probabilities of the eight teams winning

should roughly add up to 100 percent.

Now let’s see what really happens in such a situation. When the 1995 National

Basketball Association championship was down to eight teams, Fox and Tversky (1998)

recruited basketball fans as research participants. Participants were asked either (1) the

probability that each team (Chicago, Indiana, Orlando, New York, Los Angeles, Phoenix,

San Antonio, and Houston) would win the championship, (2) the probability that

the winning team would come from each of the four divisions (Central [Chicago and

Indiana], Atlantic [Orlando and New York], Pacific [Los Angeles and Phoenix], and

Midwestern [San Antonio and Houston]), or (3) the probability that the winning team

would come from either the Eastern conference (comprising the Central and Atlantic

divisions) or the Western conference (comprising the Pacific and Midwestern divisions).

If the participants were well calibrated, the sum of the probabilities for the eight

teams, the sum of the probabilities for the four divisions, and the sum of the probabilities

for the two conferences should each add up to 100 percent.

The combined probabilities for the two conferences were close to the expected 100

percent; the sum added up to 102 percent. However, the sum of the probabilities of the

four divisions was 144 percent, and the sum of the probabilities of the eight teams was

218 percent. Fox and Tversky argue that when participants focus on an individual team,

they can find reasons to support that team winning the tournament; meanwhile, the

data that support other teams winning is out of focus. Similarly, Tversky and Koehler

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!