12.12.2022 Views

BazermanMoore

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Bounded Ethicality 127

implicit desires win out. We discount the future, as well as future generations, in ways

inconsistent with our explicit environmental attitudes.

Research documents extremely high discount rates regarding the future

(Bazerman, Wade-Benzoni, & Benzoni, 1996; Loewenstein & Thaler, 1989). Most

homeowners do not sufficiently insulate their attics and walls. They also fail to buy

more expensive, energy-efficient appliances, even when they would recoup the extra

costs in less than a year. Organizations are also guilty of discounting the future. Many

institutions fail to use building materials that would be the most cost-efficient over the

long term, because of a shortsighted concern for immediate costs of construction. Investments

in efficient building materials can pay off handsomely (Ager & Dawes,

1965), yet many institutions seek to minimize the short-term cost of construction, to

the long-term detriment of their maintenance costs and the planet’s scarce resources.

Implicit Attitudes

Most people think of their attitudes, including their attitudes toward various races, as

within the scope of their conscious awareness and under their control. This view is

challenged by research on implicit attitudes, which shows, for instance, that when we

meet someone, our minds automatically activate stereotypes of the race, sex, and age of

that person (Macrae & Bodenhausen, 2001). Even people who believe strongly in egalitarian

values cannot help but have unflattering stereotypes come to mind without conscious

awareness or intent. For example, Bargh, Chen, and Burrows (1996) had

participants in their experiment work on a boring computer task. Meanwhile, the computers

flashed subliminal images of either white or black faces, so quickly that participants

were not consciously aware of them. When the computers ‘‘broke down’’ and

announced that all of the participants’ work had been lost, those who had been shown

black faces responded with significantly more aggression than those shown white faces,

consistent with the common stereotype of African-Americans as aggressive and violent.

The existence of automatic or unconscious attitudes and their effects on our judgments

can place important bounds on the degree to which we can ensure that our own

behavior is consistent with the ethical values we want to express. Again, evidence shows

that human ethicality is bounded. People often provide sincere and strenuous denials

that they intended to behave in ways consistent with negative racial stereotypes. Nevertheless,

their explicit intentions are contradicted by the implications of their actions.

Research by Jennifer Richeson and her colleagues shows that Americans of

European ancestry are often less comfortable in their interactions with Americans of

African ancestry than with Americans of European ancestry (Richeson & Shelton,

2005; Shelton, Richeson, & Vorauer, 2006). While those of European ancestry do not

intend to behave poorly toward African-Americans, they sometimes display clear psychological

signals of discomfort. Indeed, those people whose automatically activated

stereotypes are the most negative suffer most in interracial interactions (Richeson &

Trawalter, 2005). In fact, the hard work of suppressing their negative racial stereotypes

produces measurable cognitive impairments on other tasks (Richeson & Shelton, 2003).

Psychologists have developed a useful tool for examining our implicit attitudes regarding

race, gender, and other human differences that are weighted with stereotypes:

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!