12.12.2022 Views

BazermanMoore

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

After showing the video the first time, we ask our students whether anyone saw

anything unusual. In a large room, it is common for just a few people to mention seeing

a woman with an umbrella. When they offer this observation, the others in the room

scoff at it. Yet, when we show the video again to demonstrate what most of the class

missed, everyone sees the woman. By focusing on one task—in this case, counting

passes—people miss very obvious information in their visual world.

Using a video in which a person in a gorilla costume walks through a basketball

game, thumping his chest, and is clearly and comically visible for more than five seconds,

Simons and Chabris (1999) have replicated Neisser’s findings. Simons provides a

series of such demonstrations on a video that can be purchased at www.viscog.com.

We find the failure to see the obvious (including our own failure the first time we

saw the video) amazing because it violates common assumptions about our visual

awareness. This phenomenon has captured the interest of cognitive and perceptual psychologists,

and has become known as inattentional blindness (Simons & Levin, 2003).

Mack and Rock (1998) provide broader evidence in perceptual experiments that people

have a tendency not to see what they are not looking for, even when they are looking

directly at it. Mack (2003) points out that inattentional blindness might cause an airplane

pilot who is attending to his controls to overlook the presence of another airplane

in his runway. Similarly, many car accidents undoubtedly result from drivers focusing

on matters other than driving, such as talking on their cell phones (Levy, Pashler, &

Boer, 2006). We believe that research on inattentional blindness provides ample evidence

against the use of cell phones while driving, and even provides the evidentiary

basis for laws to prevent such use.

Recent work connects inattentional blindness to neural regions in the brain (Moore

& Egeth, 1997), and identifies many key independent variables that affect the probability

of not seeing the obvious (Mack, 2003). Beyond our own fascination with this basic research,

we are interested in making an analogy from this work in the visual realm to the

inattentional blindness that leads most decision makers to overlook a broad array of information

that is readily available in the environment. For instance, we are struck by the

many times our spouses have claimed to have told us something of which we have absolutely

no recollection. Like many people would, we tend to conclude that our spouses

must have imagined the interaction. But if we could miss seeing the woman with the

umbrella in Neisser’s video, we must accept the possibility that our spouses did indeed

provide the information that they claimed and that our minds were focused elsewhere.

CHANGE BLINDNESS

Change Blindness 47

Some of the most surprising studies of change blindness examine visual perception.

Change detection researchers have provided evidence that, in a surprisingly large number

of cases, people fail to notice visual changes in their physical environments

(Simons, 2000). For example, Simons, Chabris, Schnur, and Levin (2002) had an experimenter

who was holding a basketball stop a pedestrian and ask for directions. While

the pedestrian was giving directions, a group of people walked between the experimenter

and the pedestrian, and one member of the group surreptitiously took the basketball

from the experimenter. After the pedestrian finished providing directions, he or

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!