12.04.2013 Views

Formar Leitores para Ler o Mundo - Leitura Gulbenkian - Fundação ...

Formar Leitores para Ler o Mundo - Leitura Gulbenkian - Fundação ...

Formar Leitores para Ler o Mundo - Leitura Gulbenkian - Fundação ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

58<br />

Roland Barthes suggests that readers need to have access to a number of<br />

codes in order to be able to decipher the message:<br />

the proairetic code<br />

the hermeneutic code<br />

the semic code<br />

the symbolic code<br />

the referential code<br />

I will in the following consider how these codes are pertinent to visual literacy<br />

and how, if in any way, they become specific in a multimodal text.<br />

The proairetic, or anticipatory code controls understanding of the plot. In<br />

order to comprehend the plot, the reader must have elementary knowledge<br />

of what a narrative is, a text that has temporal and causal components.<br />

In using proairatic decoding, readers interact with texts on the<br />

basis of anticipation and retrospection, as developed in Wolfgang Iser’s<br />

reception theory. The understanding of plot components, such as exposition,<br />

complication, and culmination, will facilitate readers’ anticipation<br />

of a conflict resolution and, in most cases, a happy ending. It will also<br />

enable readers to make inferences from completed events about previously<br />

planted details; or to understand the characters’ actions and intentions.<br />

The most essential code in reading a picturebook is its sequential nature.<br />

This is something that art critics often ignore in their picturebook analyses.<br />

Unlike the verbal text, images are disjunctive, and it must be understood<br />

that there is a temporal and causal relationship between them. In<br />

other words, an event in one image takes place before or after an event in<br />

another image and usually is connected to it by some form of cause-and-<br />

-effect. On the other hand, unlike a verbal text, images have the possibility<br />

of depicting two or more events, suggesting <strong>para</strong>llel plots that either<br />

take place simultaneously or successively, for instance, in Come Away from<br />

the Water, Shirley, by John Burningham. The readers’ competence will<br />

decide whether they will be able to make the connection between plots<br />

or conclude that they are independent of each other.<br />

The materiality of a picturebook creates further premises. A specific feature<br />

of visual anticipatory code is page-turning, with a conspicuous<br />

detail placed in the bottom right corner of a doublespread and leading<br />

into the next one. Page layout can contribute to plot progression with several<br />

panels on a doublespread. Words and images can enhance and complement<br />

each other: words can lead readers’ attention toward images, for<br />

instance in Curious George, by H.A. Rey, where the words point at images<br />

without actually naming what happens: «First this –/and then this!» The<br />

sequential nature of a picturebook demands following plot development<br />

over several doublespreads. The visual plot can start already on the cover;<br />

it can also be developed on title page and the endpapers, and conclude<br />

on the back cover. In other words, the proairatic visual and multimodal<br />

code offers some further potential as compared to purely verbal.<br />

Images can range within a broad continuum of representation modes,<br />

from photography to abstraction. Visual literacy is here built on the<br />

understanding of the connection between the signifier and the signified,<br />

that is the word and the image. For instance, a wide range of different<br />

images of cats have the same referent, but the viewers are required to<br />

understand that the signifiers, the images, can be of different kinds. They<br />

are further prompted to ascribe higher degree of fact to photographic<br />

and true-to-life images, while abstract or distorted images will be perceived<br />

as fictional.<br />

Images can also be totally devoid of a referent, that is, be empty signifiers.<br />

Picturebooks frequently play with such possibilities, either offering a<br />

non-existing referent, such as the Grinch or a Zizzer-Zazzer-Zuzz in books<br />

by Dr. Seuss, or just ignoring the whole issue. Such books are normally<br />

labelled as nonsense, while they are in fact incredibly useful implements<br />

in visual education.<br />

The hermeneutic code, adhering closely to the proairetic, involves interpretation<br />

on the story level. The specific aspect of interpretation of a multimodal<br />

text is precisely its multimodality, where the overall meaning is<br />

created on at least two levels; moreover, not by the sum, but the interaction<br />

and synergy of these. This is a basic premise of contemporary picturebook<br />

scholarship, so I will not dwell on it further, referring to the<br />

above-mentioned works. The famous hermeneutic circle can be perfectly<br />

elucidated by the process of reading a picturebook, turning from words<br />

to images and back, with a new and deeper understanding with every spiral.<br />

Further, since images are non-linear they allow a truly hermeneutic<br />

activity of starting with the first overall impression, examining the<br />

details, returning to the whole with a more profound understanding,<br />

and so on, infinitely. Thus the understanding of the plot, of what actually<br />

happens in the narrative, is highly dependent on the level of visual<br />

sophistication. The verbal and visual plots can be mutually redundant,<br />

complementary or contradictory; they may even be independent of each<br />

other. There may be several visual plots to one verbal or several verbal<br />

plots to one visual. The verbal plot may be simple, while the visual is complex,<br />

and the other way round. The potentials are endless.<br />

In wordless picturebooks, a type of narrative that has no <strong>para</strong>llel in the<br />

novel, plots are vague and allow multiple interpretations, even when<br />

images are relatively simple. From a sequence of panels on the same doublespread<br />

or from a sequence of pages and spreads, the plot may seem<br />

more or less clear. For instance, in Jan Ormerod’s Moonlight, the most obvious<br />

plot is a child’s reluctance to go to sleep and the parents’ patience<br />

59

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!