11.07.2015 Views

View the meeting handbook - Linguistic Society of America

View the meeting handbook - Linguistic Society of America

View the meeting handbook - Linguistic Society of America

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Marc Ettlinger (University <strong>of</strong> California, Berkeley) Session 7Amy Finn (University <strong>of</strong> California, Berkeley)Carla Hudson Kam (University <strong>of</strong> California, Berkeley)The effects <strong>of</strong> sonority on word segmentationTransitional probability, phonotactics, and stress all have been shown to play a part in <strong>the</strong> segmentation <strong>of</strong> speech into words. Weinvestigate whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> sonority hierarchy also plays a role. By pitting transitional probabilities against complex onsets that adherewith varying degree to <strong>the</strong> sonority sequencing principle (SSP; Jespersen 1904), we found that violations <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> SSP led listeners tosegment words differently than <strong>the</strong>y would have using transitional probabilities alone. This suggests that in addition to <strong>the</strong> learners'existing lexicon (Pitt 1998, Samuel 1986, Moreton 2002), certain critical acoustic universals also effect how a learner perceives andacquires language.Cleveland Kent Evans (Bellevue University) Session 77From Jose Maria to Axel & Alondra: Hispanic popular culture & given names in <strong>the</strong> United StatesRecent increases in <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> certain names show that Spanish language media have a big impact on what Spanish-speaking parents in<strong>the</strong> United States choose to name <strong>the</strong>ir children. A good indication <strong>of</strong> major media impact on a name is <strong>the</strong> "tsunami curve," where asudden sharp increase peaks after only one or two years and <strong>the</strong>n begins to recede. Statistics on several names popularized bycharacters in telenovellas or by celebrities, such as actresses and athletes, give striking examples <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> impact <strong>of</strong> popular culture onparents' choices <strong>of</strong> given names in <strong>the</strong> Hispanic-<strong>America</strong>n community.Cleveland Kent Evans (Bellevue University)From Shelby to Cohen: Seventy years <strong>of</strong> popular culture influence on <strong>America</strong>n given namesANS Presidential AddressOver <strong>the</strong> past 70 years <strong>the</strong> names <strong>of</strong> popular film and television characters, "reality" program contestants, popular singers or songs,athletes, newscasters, and even murder victims have influenced what <strong>America</strong>n parents name <strong>the</strong>ir children. Though it is incorrect toexplain all changes in name popularity by reference to particular media events, especially among <strong>the</strong> very most common names,names that fit <strong>the</strong> criteria <strong>of</strong> "different but not too different" can <strong>of</strong>ten have striking increases in use when <strong>the</strong>y are presented tomillions <strong>of</strong> expectant parents at <strong>the</strong> same time through <strong>the</strong> modern mass media.Caleb Everett (University at Buffalo, State University <strong>of</strong> New York) Session 2The perception <strong>of</strong> nasality in KaritianaI present experimental data suggesting that in Tupí-Karitiana /b/ and /d/, previously considered allophones <strong>of</strong> /m/ and /n/, are attainingphonemic status. The data suggest that [b] and [d] exhibit greater perceptual distance, with respect to <strong>the</strong>ir homorganic nasalcounterparts, than would be expected if <strong>the</strong>se sounds were merely allophones <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nasals in question. The perception data areconsistent with subtle distribution patterns. This study builds upon related speech perception studies such as Harnsberger 2001 andHuang 2004, which also demonstrate that heightened levels <strong>of</strong> distributional contrast between sounds correlates with greaterperceptual distance between <strong>the</strong> sounds.Thórhallur Eythórsson (University <strong>of</strong> Iceland) Session 33The new passive in Icelandic really is a passiveA syntactic change currently underway in Icelandic involves <strong>the</strong> so-called new passive, containing an auxiliary be and a nonagreeingpast participle assigning accusative case to a postverbal argument. Contra Maling and Sigurjónsdóttir 2002, I argue that thisconstruction is a passive without NP-movement but with structural accusative case assignment. The absence <strong>of</strong> structural accusativecase assignment in <strong>the</strong> canonical passive and its presence in <strong>the</strong> new passive is attributed to parametric variation in a case feature in afunctional head taking a VP complement. Thus, <strong>the</strong> difference between <strong>the</strong> canonical passive and <strong>the</strong> new passive in Icelandic isminimal.Paul D. Fallon (University <strong>of</strong> Mary Washington) Session 38Reconstructing glottalized obstruents for Proto-Central Cushitic (Proto-Agaw)Appleyard 2006 claims that "glottalized consonants do not need to be reconstructed for Proto-Agaw" (PA) since glottalization is dueto Ethiopian Semitic languages. Examination <strong>of</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r Cushitic languages reveals data suggesting that glottalized consonants shouldbe part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> inventory <strong>of</strong> PA. A reconstruction with ejectives also yields more plausible sound changes. A change from velar118

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!