11.07.2015 Views

View the meeting handbook - Linguistic Society of America

View the meeting handbook - Linguistic Society of America

View the meeting handbook - Linguistic Society of America

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

structures to <strong>the</strong> double object construction and <strong>the</strong> prepositional construction (Marantz 1993, Harley 2003 among o<strong>the</strong>rs). I departfrom <strong>the</strong> uniform polysemy tradition in that (1) I argue that <strong>the</strong>re are two distinct sources for to-datives. (2) I allow applicative tolicense both DP and PP complements, as long as <strong>the</strong>y bear <strong>the</strong> same <strong>the</strong>ta role. By respecting UTAH (Baker 1988) and allowingvariation in categorial structure, my approach captures <strong>the</strong> fact that <strong>the</strong>matic role configurations map directly to syntax.Donna B. Gerdts (Simon Fraser University) Session 105The semantics <strong>of</strong> reciprocity in HalkomelemHalkomelem reciprocals <strong>of</strong>ten have a strong 'each o<strong>the</strong>r' meaning, allowing all permutations <strong>of</strong> agents and patients, includingadjacency, pairwise, melee, and chained meanings. Halkomelem also has asymmetric reciprocals, with a singular subject and anoblique-marked co-argument, and reciprocal unergative verbs with a 'toge<strong>the</strong>r' reading. Often, several reciprocal verbs appear in achain. The range <strong>of</strong> meanings for Halkomelem reciprocals is so broad that, ra<strong>the</strong>r than thinking <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>m as part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> domain <strong>of</strong>anaphora (as one would for English each o<strong>the</strong>r), it is more useful to discuss reciprocity in terms <strong>of</strong> event structure, as one wouldpluractionality.Jürgen Gerhards (Free University <strong>of</strong> Berlin) Session 72Denis Huschka (German Institute for Economic Research, Berlin)Gert G. Wagner (Berlin University <strong>of</strong> Technology)Naming differences in divided GermanyWe present <strong>the</strong> results <strong>of</strong> an analysis <strong>of</strong> different naming in East and West Germany. As a consequence <strong>of</strong> World War II, Germanywas territorially divided. This division lasted 40 years, a time span in which highly different geo-political frameworks influencedpeoples lives and, eventually, name choices as well. The questions are: To what extent can we, regardless <strong>of</strong> a common language anda shared cultural history, observe different name distribution patterns and name preferences in <strong>the</strong> two parts <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> country? What do<strong>the</strong> differences look like? And how did <strong>the</strong> differences develop over time?Carrie Gillon (University <strong>of</strong> British Columbia) Session 51Determiners as domain restriction: Evidence from Skwxwú7meshI propose a strict correlation between syntax and semantics, whereby <strong>the</strong> position D is universally associated with domain restriction(cf. Westerståhl 1984). Evidence for this comes from two unrelated languages: English and Skwxwú7mesh Salish. This correlationbetween D and its meaning allows us to explore <strong>the</strong> difference between Skwxwú7mesh and English. English nominals display adefinite/indefinite split whereas Skwxwú7mesh nominals do not. I argue that definiteness is not a primitive <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> grammar andinstead arises from <strong>the</strong> interaction <strong>of</strong> domain restriction and assertion <strong>of</strong> uniqueness.Ives Goddard (Smithsonian Institution) Session 101Contamination effects <strong>of</strong> two Mahican morphological changes(1) In Mahican (Eastern Algonquian), imperative singulars would be expected to have -n (< PA *-ro) in some forms and -h (< *-nro)in o<strong>the</strong>rs, but -h has replaced -n in all forms by ordinary paradigmatic analogy. By contamination, obviative singular and inanimateplural suffixes ending in -n (< *-ri) replaced this in some endings with -h by contamination from <strong>the</strong> imperative. (2) When <strong>the</strong>contraction <strong>of</strong> aw- to o was replaced by contraction to a, this replacement <strong>of</strong> surface * o by a spread by contamination to grammaticalcategories and words where it was nei<strong>the</strong>r phonologically nor morphologically motivated.Shelome Gooden (University <strong>of</strong> Pittsburgh) Session 61Maeve Eberhardt (University <strong>of</strong> Pittsburgh)AAVE in Pittsburgh: Ethnicity, local identity, & local speechWe investigate <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> features <strong>of</strong> Pittsburgh speech by African <strong>America</strong>ns in <strong>the</strong> region, focusing on two variables--<strong>the</strong>monophthongization <strong>of</strong> /aw/ and <strong>the</strong> low-back merger, which differ in <strong>the</strong>ir salience in <strong>the</strong> region. Data analyzed are drawn fromsociolinguistic interviews conducted with African <strong>America</strong>ns in Pittsburgh from three age groups. Findings reveal that whereasspeakers reject "whiteness" through avoidance <strong>of</strong> high-salient features, <strong>the</strong>re is not simultaneous rejection <strong>of</strong> "localness" since <strong>the</strong>African <strong>America</strong>n interviewees not only self-identify as ‘Pittsburghers' but also use less salient features <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> local dialect in <strong>the</strong>irown speech.124

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!