11.07.2015 Views

View the meeting handbook - Linguistic Society of America

View the meeting handbook - Linguistic Society of America

View the meeting handbook - Linguistic Society of America

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Françoise Rose (CNRS-IRD) Session 103Antoine Guillaume ( CNRS/Lumière University, Lyon 2)‘Sociative causative’ markers in South-<strong>America</strong>n languages: A possible areal featureSociative causative (aka comitative causative or causative <strong>of</strong> involvement) is a semantic type <strong>of</strong> causative where <strong>the</strong> causer not onlymakes <strong>the</strong> causee do an action, but also participates in it (Shibatani & Pardeshi 2002). This type <strong>of</strong> causative function is most <strong>of</strong>tenconveyed by a causative morpheme also coding direct or indirect causation. In many South <strong>America</strong>n languages, however, thiscategory is expressed by a specific morpheme, which leads us to hypo<strong>the</strong>size that a specific marker for sociative causative could be anareal feature <strong>of</strong> South <strong>America</strong>n (or maybe more globally <strong>of</strong> Amerindian) languages.Mary Rose (Ohio State University) Session 49Never around <strong>the</strong> barns: Gendered linguistic practices in dairy countryVariation research has begun to examine <strong>the</strong> ideologies and stances mediating between linguistic resources and social categories,especially gender. I discuss gender and class distinctions in a cluster <strong>of</strong> phonetic variables deployed by older speakers in ruralWisconsin. Data from ethnographic interviews with 36 speakers aged 66-99 support <strong>the</strong> analysis <strong>of</strong> several phonetic variables: (dh)-fortition, (ow)-raising and monophthongization, and (ey)-raising.I examine <strong>the</strong>se sociophonetic resources along with narrative practices, leisure activities, and patterns <strong>of</strong> social interaction to showhow all speakers, but especially women, maintain <strong>the</strong> stylistic distinctions constituting <strong>the</strong> community's social and spatial landscape.Ca<strong>the</strong>rine Rudin (Wayne State College) Session 39Multiple wh-fronting in correlatives & free relativesMultiple wh-fronting (MWF) exhibits differences between free relatives and correlatives and between two types <strong>of</strong> languages,exemplified by Bulgarian and Polish. Both languages have MWF relatives, but Polish has only MWF correlatives while Bulgarian hasboth MWF correlatives and MWF free relatives. The difference in availability <strong>of</strong> MWF is attributable to differing positions <strong>of</strong> frontedwh-words in <strong>the</strong> two languages: Multiple free relatives but not multiple correlatives require a structure with all wh-words in SpecCP.MWF relatives thus exactly parallel <strong>the</strong> structure <strong>of</strong> MWF questions in a given language, as fur<strong>the</strong>r indicated by superiority facts.Jeffrey T. Runner (University <strong>of</strong> Rochester) Session 45Micah B. Goldwater (University <strong>of</strong> Texas, Austin)Reference transfer & reflexive interpretation in representational noun phrasesThe main explanations for <strong>the</strong> exceptional behavior <strong>of</strong> reflexives in ‘representational NPs’ (RNPs) rely on syntactic or argumentstructure (Chomsky 1986, Davies & Dubinsky 2003, Pollard & Sag 1992, Reinhart & Reuland 1993). ‘Reference transfer’ (RT)allows reference to a representation <strong>of</strong> a person by that person's name (Jackend<strong>of</strong>f 1992). Like RNP reflexives (Grodzinsky &Reinhart 1993), RT reflexives may receive coreferential interpretations when elided (Lidz 2001). We present evidence from twopicture verification experiments and one eye-tracking experiment that it is <strong>the</strong> representational use <strong>of</strong> RNP reflexives--and not <strong>the</strong>syntactic/argument structure--that allows for <strong>the</strong>ir exceptional behavior.C. Anton Rytting (Ohio State University) Session 11Chris Brew (Ohio State University)Eric Fosler-Lussier (Ohio State University)Modeling word segmentation without assuming phonemic certaintyMost computational models <strong>of</strong> word segmentation assume unrealistic degrees <strong>of</strong> invariance in <strong>the</strong> input provided to infants. This workpresents one such model <strong>of</strong> word segmentation (Christiansen et al., 1998) with input automatically derived from speech--more closelyapproximating <strong>the</strong> auditory input available to infants--and suggests that <strong>the</strong> modeled use <strong>of</strong> segmental cues is less robust to inputvariability than previously thought. A simple modification <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> model improves its performance on variable data.Ivan Sag (Stanford University) Session 44Philip H<strong>of</strong>meister (Stanford University)Neal Snider (Stanford University)Perry Rosenstein (Stanford University)Controlling processing factors in <strong>the</strong> study <strong>of</strong> subjacencyWe report on our investigations <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> complex NP constraint (CNPC) or ‘Subjacency' effects in so-called fact-that clauses. We160

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!