11.07.2015 Views

View the meeting handbook - Linguistic Society of America

View the meeting handbook - Linguistic Society of America

View the meeting handbook - Linguistic Society of America

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Jonathan Howell (Cornell University) Session 18Second occurrence focus & <strong>the</strong> acoustics <strong>of</strong> prominencePartee 1991 challenged <strong>the</strong> significance <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> observation that certain adverbs (e.g. only) reliably ‘associate’ with phonologicallyprominent words to truth-conditional effect. She noted second occurrence focus appears to lack a phonological realization, e.g. (1).Several recent studies have suggested that <strong>the</strong> focus is realized by o<strong>the</strong>r cues including duration and intensity.(1) a. Johnson only PEDDLES pedals lately.b. Even THOMPSON only peddles pedals lately.I report on a small production study showing that, contra many assumptions, a simple duration difference is not a straightforwardindication <strong>of</strong> semantic focus. I address spectral cues and perception.Yuchau E. Hsiao (National Chengchi University) Session 31The rhythmic structure <strong>of</strong> Taiwan folk verseA topic that has recently attracted much attention is <strong>the</strong> grammar <strong>of</strong> metrics. I discuss <strong>the</strong> rhythm <strong>of</strong> Taiwan folk verse, based on acorpus <strong>of</strong> 2,648 lines. Unlike <strong>the</strong> classical verses, where every syllable receives a beat, <strong>the</strong> folk verses allow two syllables to share abeat and allow grammatical words to contrast rhythmically. Some <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> folk verse lines may have alternative readings--one derivedby one-to-one mapping, while <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r by beat sharing. I posit a set <strong>of</strong> constraints to govern <strong>the</strong> beat assignments under OT andexplore a general <strong>the</strong>ory <strong>of</strong> meter.Sarah Hulsey (Masachusetts Institute <strong>of</strong> Technology) Session 56Distributed modal readings in gapping sentencesThere is a conjunction/disjunction asymmetry in gapping sentences containing a modal. Conjunction always takes narrow scoperelative to <strong>the</strong> modal; disjunction may take ei<strong>the</strong>r narrow- or wide-scope. I adopt a Hamblin semantics for disjunction: or is not aBoolean operator, but introduces a set <strong>of</strong> alternatives; <strong>the</strong> modal can distribute pointwise over <strong>the</strong> Alt-set, giving <strong>the</strong> distributed modalreading. In contrast, I analyze conjunction as a traditional Boolean operation (defined for sets). This analysis makes predictions for<strong>the</strong> distribution <strong>of</strong> various modals in negated gapping sentences.Hyekyung Hwang (University <strong>of</strong> Hawaii, Manoa) Session 54Amy J. Schafer (University <strong>of</strong> Hawaii, Manoa)Length effects in <strong>the</strong> resolution <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> dative NP ambiguity in KoreanPrevious reports on whe<strong>the</strong>r implicit prosody influences syntactic disambiguation in silent reading have been mixed. While Hirose2003 found phrase length effects in Japanese, Jun and Kim 2004 did not find an effect <strong>of</strong> relative clause length on prosodic phrasing inproduction in Korean, although RC length did affect <strong>of</strong>f-line perception. We examine whe<strong>the</strong>r and how <strong>the</strong> length <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> matrixsubject affects a following dative NP's on-line attachment preferences in Korean sentences. Our results indicate effects <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>preceding phrase length on dative NP attachment during silent reading, supporting <strong>the</strong> implicit prosody hypo<strong>the</strong>sis (Fodor 1998,2002).Jiwon Hwang (University at Stony Brook, State University <strong>of</strong> New York) Session 55Ellen Broselow (University at Stony Brook, State University <strong>of</strong> New York)Susana de Leon (University at Stony Brook, State University <strong>of</strong> New York)Nancy Squires (University at Stony Brook, State University <strong>of</strong> New York)Minimizing <strong>the</strong> distance between perception & productionWe report on results <strong>of</strong> behavioral and ERP studies <strong>of</strong> perception in which Korean listeners were presented with pairs <strong>of</strong> stimuli alonga continuum that ranged from no vowel (e.g. tegnal) to a full vowel (e.g. tegnal) at intervals <strong>of</strong> 20msec. This experiment revealed that<strong>the</strong> boundary for categorical perception <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> /stop-nasal/ vs /stopV-nasal/ varies by place. We propose that Korean speakers'tendency to insert a vowel more <strong>of</strong>ten in /gN/ sequences than in /bN/ sequences is due to this perceptual asymmetry.Larry M. Hyman (University <strong>of</strong> California, Berkeley) Session 31There is no pitch-accent prototypeMany scholars use <strong>the</strong> term ‘pitch-accent’ to refer to a defective tone system whose mark is obligatory, culminative, privative,131

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!