12.07.2015 Views

Art Un ticle I.1 ited Sta In the ates News - Woodring College of ...

Art Un ticle I.1 ited Sta In the ates News - Woodring College of ...

Art Un ticle I.1 ited Sta In the ates News - Woodring College of ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS
  • No tags were found...

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

<strong>the</strong> students. Ano<strong>the</strong>r common fallacy is that acquiring more than one language is “difficult” andcan lead to academic problems. Some teachers have been heard to suggest that <strong>the</strong>ir bilingualstudents don’t speak any language to a real extent and are “semilingual.” We will return to <strong>the</strong>semyths at <strong>the</strong> end <strong>of</strong> this chapter. By <strong>the</strong>n, <strong>the</strong> intervening information will have provided youwith enough understanding about <strong>the</strong> normal process <strong>of</strong> language acquisition that <strong>the</strong> faulty logic<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se beliefs should be obvious.Regardless <strong>of</strong> whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> above beliefs lack grounding, <strong>the</strong>y are very common and caninfluence how educators assess and educate CLD students. Without understanding how languageacquisition works, <strong>the</strong> continuation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se fallacies is understandable. Therefore, educatorsworking with bilingual students must understand <strong>the</strong> language acquisition process for effectiveimplementation <strong>of</strong> assessment and selection <strong>of</strong> appropriate intervention techniques.Language AcquisitionTheories <strong>of</strong> Language AcquisitionWhy do we study language acquisition? Depending on <strong>the</strong> orientation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> researcher, <strong>the</strong> study<strong>of</strong> language acquisition can be ei<strong>the</strong>r important in and <strong>of</strong> itself or as a mechanism forunderstanding o<strong>the</strong>r areas <strong>of</strong> research such as <strong>the</strong>oretical linguistics, cognitive psychology,neuroscience, or child development. Some want to know what <strong>the</strong> process <strong>of</strong> first and secondlanguage acquisition tell us about human development, learning, and organization <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> brain.O<strong>the</strong>rs want to know about language acquisition so <strong>the</strong>y can make recommendations abouteducational programs. Regardless <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir research focus, all <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se researchers must take aposition on some fundamental arguments about <strong>the</strong> development <strong>of</strong> language and its relationshipto o<strong>the</strong>r mental functions.Linguistic <strong>the</strong>ory can be divided into several main camps depending on 1) whe<strong>the</strong>r language isviewed as ei<strong>the</strong>r separate from or as an outgrowth <strong>of</strong> cognition and 2) whe<strong>the</strong>r languageacquisition is believed to be directed by a preprogrammed, language-specific area <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> brain orwhe<strong>the</strong>r it is guided primarily by experiential factors. Noam Chomsky addressed both <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>seissues when he proposed that human brains are “hardwired” for language acquisition via whathas come to be called <strong>the</strong> “Language Acquisition Device” or LAD (Chomsky, 1965). This isconsidered a “nativist” <strong>the</strong>oretical position. The development <strong>of</strong> this <strong>the</strong>ory was in directopposition to B. F. Skinner’s ideas about <strong>the</strong> development <strong>of</strong> language as shaped by behavioralreinforcement received from a child’s environment (Skinner, 1957). Chomsky demonstrated that<strong>the</strong> language available in a child’s environment is far too complex and <strong>the</strong> reinforcement (i.e.praise) far too inconsistent to account for a behavioral model <strong>of</strong> language development. Although<strong>the</strong> extent to which grammatical structures are specifically preprogrammed is still hotly debated(Bowerman, 1994), most child language specialists today agree that, at least to some extent, <strong>the</strong>human brain is predisposed to 1) attend differentially to language input, 2) process that inputaccording to some preset principles, and 3) formulate unconscious rules for languagecomprehension and production.This perspective <strong>of</strong> language as biologically driven has been traditionally considereddiametrically opposed to sociocultural perspectives that recognize <strong>the</strong> role <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> environment inshaping language development. However, many child language researchers are coming to <strong>the</strong>conclusion that this does not necessarily need to be <strong>the</strong> case. Some have come to take <strong>the</strong>position that “it is perfectly consistent to believe that, while much <strong>of</strong> language development isgoverned by <strong>the</strong> operation <strong>of</strong> powerful innate principles, some important aspects <strong>of</strong> earlylanguage development are significantly influenced by <strong>the</strong> child’s language experience” (Harris,© 2008 Dr. Ca<strong>the</strong>rine CollierAll Rights Reserved84

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!