12.07.2015 Views

Art Un ticle I.1 ited Sta In the ates News - Woodring College of ...

Art Un ticle I.1 ited Sta In the ates News - Woodring College of ...

Art Un ticle I.1 ited Sta In the ates News - Woodring College of ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS
  • No tags were found...

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

1992, p. XI). These researchers, while acknowledging <strong>the</strong> existence <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> biological basis <strong>of</strong>language acquisition, also acknowledge that “interpretation and meaning are necessarilyembedded in cultural systems <strong>of</strong> understanding. If language is a meaning-making system andspeaking and listening are meaning-making activities, <strong>the</strong>n accounts <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se phenomena must atsome point draw on accounts <strong>of</strong> society and culture” (Ochs, 1988, p. 4). <strong>In</strong> recognition <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>influence <strong>of</strong> culture, Schieffelin and Ochs (1986) refer to <strong>the</strong> process <strong>of</strong> language acquisition in<strong>the</strong> sociocultural context as “language socialization.” Ochs defines socialization as “<strong>the</strong> processby which one becomes a competent member or society” (p. 5) and language socialization as“socialization through language and socialization to use language” (p.14). This perspectiverecognizes that linguistic competence involves much more than <strong>the</strong> ability to comprehend andproduce grammatically correct utterances. Language competence involves <strong>the</strong> ability to selectbetween a variety <strong>of</strong> possible options <strong>of</strong> linguistic form and content according <strong>the</strong> social contextand cultural norms, as well as interpreting subtle meanings that require extensive social andcultural knowledge. An example is knowing when “Would you like to sit down?” becomes anindirect command, a real question, or a comment on inappropriate behavior.The study <strong>of</strong> second language acquisition clearly fits within <strong>the</strong> above <strong>the</strong>oretical debate.<strong>Un</strong>derstanding how second language learners best acquire a new language may shed moreinformation about <strong>the</strong> relative influence <strong>of</strong> cognition, language-specific brain functions, andenvironmental influences. <strong>Un</strong>like <strong>the</strong> process <strong>of</strong> first language acquisition in young children, <strong>the</strong>process <strong>of</strong> second language acquisition can be observed under a variety <strong>of</strong> circumstances. Therelative influences <strong>of</strong> age, personality, social context, type <strong>of</strong> language input and o<strong>the</strong>r factorscan be better isolated and explored. This information can, in turn, inform our understanding <strong>of</strong>how language acquisition as a generic process unfolds.The Process <strong>of</strong> First Language AcquisitionIt is a remarkable fact that, without special training or carefully sequenced linguistic input,every normal child acquires a natural language. The universality <strong>of</strong> language in our speciesstands in glaring contrast to <strong>the</strong> much more selective attainment <strong>of</strong> comparable cognitive skills,such as <strong>the</strong> ability to perform arithmetic calculations. A related fact is that every child in alinguistic community succeeds in converging on a grammatical system that is equivalent toeveryone else’s, despite considerable variability in linguistic experience. Moreover, childrenacquire language quite rapidly and with few wrong turns, considering <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> potentialpitfalls that exist (Crain, 1994, p. 364).Precursors to language production.Language acquisition is indeed a remarkable process. Children acquire <strong>the</strong> language(s) heardaround <strong>the</strong>m and seem to do so in strikingly similar ways <strong>the</strong> world over. This process is evenmore remarkable when we consider what a child must know and be able to do to produce herfirst word, for example, “mama.” To answer this question, we need to distinguish between <strong>the</strong>real use <strong>of</strong> a word for communicative purposes and <strong>the</strong> imitation <strong>of</strong> a string <strong>of</strong> sounds. It is easyfor first time parents, eagerly anticipating Joey’s first word, to interpret /mama/ as mo<strong>the</strong>r and/dada/ for daddy when <strong>the</strong> child is really just engaging in babbling or sound play. <strong>In</strong> order to call/mama/ or /dada/ a word, it should occur in consistent contexts, spontaneously (withoutimitation), and appear to indicate a communicative intent. Clearly, this can be difficult todetermine. However, if Joey says /dada/ when repeating sounds made during play with hismo<strong>the</strong>r and produces this consonant-vowel combination along with o<strong>the</strong>rs, such as /baba/, /gaga/,and /dadada/, <strong>the</strong>n we can probably suspect that it is not a real word. On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, if Joey© 2008 Dr. Ca<strong>the</strong>rine CollierAll Rights Reserved85

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!