12.07.2015 Views

Art Un ticle I.1 ited Sta In the ates News - Woodring College of ...

Art Un ticle I.1 ited Sta In the ates News - Woodring College of ...

Art Un ticle I.1 ited Sta In the ates News - Woodring College of ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS
  • No tags were found...

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

development. This research described children’s level <strong>of</strong> language development according to ameasure <strong>of</strong> utterance length - Mean Length <strong>of</strong> Utterance (MLU). A determination <strong>of</strong> MLU ismade by analyzing a 100 utterance language sample for <strong>the</strong> average length <strong>of</strong> child-producedutterances, in terms <strong>of</strong> morphemes. For example, <strong>the</strong> utterance 'Bobby hitting' has a morphemecount <strong>of</strong> 3, as 'hitting' includes both <strong>the</strong> verb 'to hit' and <strong>the</strong> present progressive morpheme -ing.This type <strong>of</strong> measure <strong>of</strong> language development, ra<strong>the</strong>r than age, is considered a better predictor<strong>of</strong> grammatical development, as children <strong>of</strong> similar chronological ages can vary greatly in <strong>the</strong>irlanguage acquisition. Therefore, researchers have attempted to determine in what order and atwhat stage <strong>of</strong> linguistic development different language forms should appear.<strong>Un</strong>til recently, most research has focused on similarities in grammatical development, with <strong>the</strong>accompanying perception that all normal children follow a relatively fixed and stable pattern <strong>of</strong>grammatical development. However, more recently, investigators have begun to question thisassumption, with research that indic<strong>ates</strong> a greater variability than previously assumed (Lahey,Liebergott, Chesnick, Menyuk & Adams, 1982). Given <strong>the</strong> original research indicating veryconsistent patterns <strong>of</strong> grammatical acquisition among middle-class native-English speakers, thistrend was assumed to hold for non-dominant culture English speakers, as well as childrenacquiring languages o<strong>the</strong>r than English.A considerable amount <strong>of</strong> research on <strong>the</strong> grammatical development <strong>of</strong> native-Spanish speakershas been performed, with <strong>the</strong> goal <strong>of</strong> finding a consistent order <strong>of</strong> acquisition <strong>of</strong> morphemes suchas been found among middle class, native English speakers (e.g. González, 1978; Kvaal,Shipstead-Cox, Nevitt, Hodson & Launer, 1988; Olarte, 1985; Vivas, 1979). However, in a metaanalysis<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se studies, Merino (1992) found that no more than very gross generalities could befound, such as <strong>the</strong> acquisition <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> present tense before <strong>the</strong> subjective. These findings haveimportant implications for <strong>the</strong> language assessment <strong>of</strong> bilingual and non-English speakingchildren.These studies that indicate that <strong>the</strong> order and developmental age <strong>of</strong> appearance <strong>of</strong> morphemesmay not be as consistent as previously thought should warn educators and assessment personnelagainst making judgments about grammatical development for <strong>the</strong> purposes <strong>of</strong> determiningspecial education placement. We need to be very cautious when making comparisons <strong>of</strong>individual children against assumed benchmarks <strong>of</strong> “normal” development. These guidelines <strong>of</strong>morphological development should be seen as generalities and deviance from this observedprogression should not be taken as indications <strong>of</strong> a language problem.<strong>In</strong> addition to research focused on <strong>the</strong> stage and sequence <strong>of</strong> grammatical development,researchers have also looked at <strong>the</strong> process by which children acquire grammatical forms. Thisprocess was assumed to be a matter <strong>of</strong> modeling, imitation and reinforcement when Skinner’sbehavioral model <strong>of</strong> language development was in vogue. However, researchers have sinceobserved that children seem to go through a process <strong>of</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>sis testing. Children may acquirea form <strong>of</strong> a word, such as “went” without acquiring <strong>the</strong> rule for irregular past tenses. Later, when<strong>the</strong>y acquire <strong>the</strong> rule for formation <strong>of</strong> regular past tenses, <strong>the</strong>y may over generalize <strong>the</strong> rule,forming productions such as “goed”. As <strong>the</strong>y fine tune <strong>the</strong>ir system <strong>of</strong> grammatical rules andacquire <strong>the</strong> irregular past tense, <strong>the</strong>y will once again correctly produce “went”. During thisperiod <strong>of</strong> over-extensions, children may be very resistant to corrections <strong>of</strong> forms that <strong>the</strong>y havenot yet acquired. Table 6-2 provides an example <strong>of</strong> a hypo<strong>the</strong>tical, but typical mo<strong>the</strong>r-childinteraction that demonstr<strong>ates</strong> children’s attention to <strong>the</strong> truthfulness, ra<strong>the</strong>r than <strong>the</strong> grammaticalcorrectness <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> input. <strong>In</strong> fact, observations <strong>of</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r-child dyads indicate that parents respond© 2008 Dr. Ca<strong>the</strong>rine CollierAll Rights Reserved88

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!