20.03.2013 Views

Second Language Acquisition and Second ... - Stephen Krashen

Second Language Acquisition and Second ... - Stephen Krashen

Second Language Acquisition and Second ... - Stephen Krashen

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

on actual use of routines <strong>and</strong> patterns in adult second language performance. There<br />

is, however, some interesting literature from scholars concerned with materials <strong>and</strong><br />

language pedagogy. Consider the "st<strong>and</strong>ard" audio-lingual technique of first<br />

memorizing a dialogue (as a routine) <strong>and</strong> then working on pattern practice. This is<br />

an implicit version of position 1: the routine is acquired from the dialogue <strong>and</strong> then<br />

broken into patterns for pattern practice, <strong>and</strong> it is suggested that the adult gains<br />

fluency through the use or transfer of these patterns to real conversational contexts.<br />

In other words, routines <strong>and</strong> patterns constitute all of language performance. (This is<br />

certainly not the view of all those scholars who encourage the use of pattern<br />

practice; see, for example, Rivers, 1972, pp. 10-12.)<br />

To be sure, audio-lingual language teaching does result in increased student<br />

proficiency in second language, although other systems seem to be more efficient<br />

(see von Elek <strong>and</strong> Oskarsson, 1973). Does this mean that position 1 is correct? Not<br />

necessarily. It may appear to be the case that routines <strong>and</strong> patterns are being learned<br />

in the classroom <strong>and</strong> used as such in performance. In reality, proficiency gained in<br />

such circumstances is more likely due to the creative construction process, which is<br />

present in both adults <strong>and</strong> children (<strong>Krashen</strong>, 1977b), as well as the learning of<br />

explicit conscious rules (hypothesized to be used only as a Monitor).<br />

First, even meaningless pattern practice may provide some, if less than optimal,<br />

input for the creative construction process. <strong>Second</strong>, such practice may also provide a<br />

domain for conscious rule learning, either as an environment for working out rules<br />

(inductive) or as a place to practice rule application (deductive) <strong>and</strong> to receive<br />

feedback.<br />

The use of routines <strong>and</strong> patterns is certainly a part of language, but it is probably not<br />

a large part. In fact, many second language performers report that it is often quite<br />

difficult to find a spot in a conversation for a routine or pattern learned in a dialogue<br />

(K. Schule, personal communication). In addition, sometimes performers report<br />

having to run through an entire dialogue to get to the appropriate line!<br />

Even if the gestalt mode per se were learned in the audiolingual class, there are<br />

dangers in encouraging adults to use it. The outside world for adults is nowhere near<br />

as predictable as the linguistic environment around Fillmore's children was. The<br />

wide use of routines<br />

98

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!