20.03.2013 Views

Second Language Acquisition and Second ... - Stephen Krashen

Second Language Acquisition and Second ... - Stephen Krashen

Second Language Acquisition and Second ... - Stephen Krashen

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>and</strong> often incorporated them into his speech. This is similar to what Clark found in<br />

her study of Adam. Wagner-Gough hypothesizes that patterns do not directly evolve<br />

into creative rule-governed language: "It is quite clear that there is no transfer<br />

between some imitations <strong>and</strong> subsequent free speech patterns. For example, a<br />

learner may say 'My name is Homer' in one breath <strong>and</strong> 'He Fred' in another, the<br />

former being a memorized pattern <strong>and</strong> the latter the learner's own rule" (p. 71).<br />

Thus, Wagner-Gough supports position 2.<br />

The most complete study of routines <strong>and</strong> patterns in child second language<br />

acquisition is L. Fillmore's doctoral dissertation (Fillmore, 1976), an exhaustive<br />

examination of the speech produced by five acquirers of English as a second<br />

language in an English-speaking kindergarten. Unlike Wagner-Gough, Fillmore<br />

comes out strongly for position 3:<br />

... the strategy of acquiring formulaic speech is central to the learning of language:<br />

indeed, it is this step that puts the learner in a position to perform the analysis which<br />

is prerequisite to acquisition. The formulas... constituted the linguistic material on<br />

which a large part of their (the children's) analytical activities could be carried out....<br />

Once they were in the learner's repertory, they became familiar, <strong>and</strong> therefore could<br />

be compared with other utterances in the repertory as well as with those produced by<br />

other speakers (p. 640).<br />

All the children studied by Fillmore used routines <strong>and</strong> patterns very early <strong>and</strong> very<br />

heavily: "The most striking similarity among the spontaneous speech records of the<br />

five children was the acquisition <strong>and</strong> use of formulaic expressions. All five quickly<br />

acquired repertoires of expression which they knew how to use more or less<br />

appropriately, <strong>and</strong> put them to immediate <strong>and</strong> frequent use (p. 640). Including only<br />

the clearest cases of formulaic expressions, Fillmore calculated that their use ranged<br />

from 52 per cent to 100 per cent of the total number of utterances at the early stages,<br />

down to a low of 37 percent in the most advanced performer at the end of the year.<br />

Two children, in fact, remained nearly completely dependent on routines <strong>and</strong><br />

patterns even at the end of the year.<br />

Routines <strong>and</strong> patterns evolved into creative language in a manner not unlike that<br />

reported by R. Clark (1974). Larger units were broken into smaller units, routines<br />

became patterns, <strong>and</strong> parts of patterns were "freed" to recombine with other parts of<br />

patterns. This break-up<br />

93

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!