20.03.2013 Views

Second Language Acquisition and Second ... - Stephen Krashen

Second Language Acquisition and Second ... - Stephen Krashen

Second Language Acquisition and Second ... - Stephen Krashen

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

which he had learned by imitation. Hatch's comment neatly summarizes the<br />

situation: "Quite clearly two separate <strong>and</strong> very distinct strategies were running side<br />

by side. After week 12 it became increasingly difficult to separate out imitations<br />

since Paul's rule stages moved so fast that he quickly caught up with the language as<br />

it was spoken by the children in the playground" (p. 31).<br />

The picture Hatch describes for Paul resembles, in reverse, what one may see in<br />

recovery patterns in aphasia. Alajouanine (1956) notes that when propositional<br />

speech returns, "fixed phrases" may disappear. The automatic speech is immune to<br />

the ungrammaticality of the aphasic's developing language.<br />

The relationship of analytic <strong>and</strong> gestalt speech in Paul is again that predicted by<br />

position 2: the two modes are independent <strong>and</strong> the analytic mode eventually<br />

predominates, with gestalt speech primarily serving only as a short-cut, a pragmatic<br />

tool to allow social interaction with a minimum of linguistic competence.<br />

Hatch suggests a reason why second language acquirers may use more routines <strong>and</strong><br />

patterns than first language acquirers. She emphasizes the second language<br />

performer's greater capacity to remember longer utterances: "The person (L2<br />

acquirer) brings with him a great capacity to create language by rule formation. At<br />

the same time he is capable of storing, repeating, <strong>and</strong> remembering large chunks of<br />

language via imitation. He can repeat them for use in an appropriate situation.<br />

While he is still at the two-word stage in rule formation, he can recall <strong>and</strong> use<br />

longer imitated sentences" (p. 33)<br />

In another case history, Hakuta (1974) reported on the linguistic development of<br />

Uguisu, a Japanese-speaking 5-year-old acquiring English as a second language in<br />

informal situations in the United States. Hakuta reports that he found evidence for<br />

"a strategy of learning on the surface structure level: learning through rote<br />

memorization of segments of speech without knowledge of the internal structure of<br />

those speech segments (p. 287).<br />

Hakuta's study is mostly concerned with patterns (as opposed to routines):<br />

"segments of sentences which operate in conjunction with a movable component,<br />

such as the insertion of a noun phrase or a verb phrase", <strong>and</strong> the evidence he<br />

provides for the existence of patterns in his corpus is quite similar to that provided<br />

by Brown (1973) in his<br />

91

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!