20.03.2013 Views

Second Language Acquisition and Second ... - Stephen Krashen

Second Language Acquisition and Second ... - Stephen Krashen

Second Language Acquisition and Second ... - Stephen Krashen

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

etween overall SLOPE scores <strong>and</strong> measures of exposure. Despite our findings that the<br />

SLOPE, as administered, is primarily an acquisition measure (because it yielded a "natural"<br />

difficulty order <strong>and</strong> allowed no monitoring time), no relationship was found between the<br />

measure of exposure <strong>and</strong> SLOPE scores.<br />

These results confirm the suspicions voiced above about using "exposure-type" measures of<br />

informal linguistic environments, <strong>and</strong> underline the claim that active involvement is necessary<br />

for acquisition<br />

Table 2. SLOPE performance <strong>and</strong> measures of exposure<br />

<strong>and</strong> formal instruction<br />

_________________________________________________________________________<br />

Years in English- Years of formal<br />

speaking country English study<br />

_________________________________________________________________________<br />

r p r p<br />

SLOPE scores 0.014 ns 0.42 p < 0.001<br />

_________________________________________________________________________<br />

Partial correlations were used, as years in English-speaking country <strong>and</strong> years of formal study<br />

were correlated, r = -0.24, p < 0.01. Ordinary correlations were computed, however, <strong>and</strong> were<br />

quite similar to those reported above; for SLOPE <strong>and</strong> exposure, r = 0.003, <strong>and</strong> for SLOPE <strong>and</strong><br />

formal study, r = 0.40.<br />

to take place. Thus, if the SLOPE is a test of acquired competence only, it must be concluded<br />

that the question asked in the <strong>Krashen</strong> et al. series is a measure of time spent in "exposure-type"<br />

environments only, <strong>and</strong> this apparent counter-evidence to hypothesis I disappears entirely. No<br />

studies in the literature survey, however, are counter-evidence to the hypothesis that an "intaketype"<br />

informal environment may be quite efficient in increasing adult second language<br />

proficiency.<br />

The significant correlation in Table 2 between years of formal instruction <strong>and</strong> SLOPE scores<br />

supports the hypothesis that the classroom can be of value, <strong>and</strong> in fact generally is of value, in<br />

language acquisition as well as in language learning.<br />

While all studies described here are consistent with a revised version of hypothesis II, that in<br />

general formal instruction increases second language proficiency, none of the studies gives<br />

evidence to indicate that "learning" does indeed take place in formal situation in addition to<br />

acquisition. Note that "learning" occurs in the Upshur <strong>and</strong> Mason studies only under the "selfstudy"<br />

hypothesis, while in the <strong>Krashen</strong> et al.<br />

48

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!