20.03.2013 Views

Second Language Acquisition and Second ... - Stephen Krashen

Second Language Acquisition and Second ... - Stephen Krashen

Second Language Acquisition and Second ... - Stephen Krashen

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

"That's a _______") which were characterized by very high initial performance.<br />

Related propositional forms, Brown notes, show a learning curve with a gradual<br />

increase in accuracy over time. Patterns <strong>and</strong> routines, on the other h<strong>and</strong>, do not.<br />

Brown is thus solidly behind position 2 for child language acquisition. Patterns <strong>and</strong><br />

routines may develop due to high frequency in input in advance of linguistic<br />

maturity, but such automatic speech is independent of the creative construction<br />

process; it may fall away in some situations <strong>and</strong> be reanalyzed by the language<br />

acquisition process.<br />

On the other h<strong>and</strong>, . Clark (1974), in a paper appropriately titled "Performing<br />

without Competence", argues that for some children routines do evolve into patterns<br />

which in turn become creative language. In her words, "our research finding suggest<br />

that (child's speech) becomes creative through the gradual analysis of the internal<br />

structure of sequences which begin as prepackaged routine" (p. 9). Clark's<br />

conclusions are based on her analysis of her son Adam (not to be confused with<br />

Brown's subject of the same name) who was about 3 years old at the time of the<br />

study. Adam, according to Clark, would often use his own or another's previous<br />

utterance as constituents for his own utterances. For example,<br />

Adam R. C.<br />

Mummy you go. Where?<br />

Mummy you go swings.<br />

Do you want to get off?<br />

No I want to get on. (This was an error: Adam meant<br />

to say "stay on".)<br />

In addition to this use of patterns, Adam tended to use routines as well, trying to use<br />

a phrase in situations close to where he heard it. For example, he would say, "I carry<br />

you" when he wanted to be carried, having heard his father say this sentence on<br />

numerous occasions.<br />

This use of routines <strong>and</strong> patterns may not be atypical. Clark, however, suggests that<br />

for Adam routines evolve into patterns, consistent with positions 1 <strong>and</strong> 3 above. For<br />

example, when a hot meal was brought to the table<br />

(1) Wait for it to cool<br />

87

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!