20.03.2013 Views

Second Language Acquisition and Second ... - Stephen Krashen

Second Language Acquisition and Second ... - Stephen Krashen

Second Language Acquisition and Second ... - Stephen Krashen

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

discussion of Adam (see discussion above). For example, copula forms are judged<br />

to be patterns due to very high learning performance, or the lack of a learning curve.<br />

Uguisu produced forms like "This is _______" accurately from the outset, while<br />

learning curves for other items (like the progressive <strong>and</strong> auxiliary morphemes) were<br />

gradual. Also, Uguisu overgeneralized the use of patterns, using patterns like<br />

"These are _____" in singular contexts much of the time. Another example is the do<br />

you phrase used as a question marker. Uguisu produced sentences like:<br />

What do you doing, boy?<br />

These forms gradually gave way to correctly inflected forms for do.<br />

In discussing why the child second language acquirer may use routines <strong>and</strong> patterns<br />

more than the first language acquirer, Hakuta emphasizes the older child's greater<br />

need to communicate: "... in the case of the second language learner, we would<br />

expect that, with advanced semantic development <strong>and</strong> yet no form with which to<br />

express such thoughts, the need to learn the various linguistic structures of the target<br />

language is especially acute." Until the structure of the language is acquired, it is<br />

conceivable, Hakuta suggests, that "the learner will employ a strategy which 'tunes<br />

in' on regular, patterned segments of speech, <strong>and</strong> employs them without knowledge<br />

of their underlying structure, but with the knowledge as to which particular<br />

situations call for what patterns. They may be thought of as props which<br />

temporarily give support until a firmer foundation is built..." (p. 288).<br />

Synthesizing both Hatch <strong>and</strong> Hakuta, one may conclude that the child second<br />

language acquirer has both an increased need <strong>and</strong> ability to use routines <strong>and</strong><br />

patterns. The child second language performer is placed in peer <strong>and</strong> school<br />

situations that dem<strong>and</strong> linguistic interaction before competence is attained the "slow<br />

way", <strong>and</strong> the older child's advanced short-term memory allows him to pick up <strong>and</strong><br />

retain the necessary formulas to facilitate interaction.<br />

Another study which examines the use of routines <strong>and</strong> patterns in child second<br />

language acquisition is that of Wagner-Gough, (1975, see also Wagner-Gough <strong>and</strong><br />

Hatch, 1975). Wagner-Gough noted that her subject Homer relied heavily on<br />

routines <strong>and</strong> patterns to communicate<br />

92

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!