20.03.2013 Views

Second Language Acquisition and Second ... - Stephen Krashen

Second Language Acquisition and Second ... - Stephen Krashen

Second Language Acquisition and Second ... - Stephen Krashen

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

in a second language, or a first language, without ever producing it. There are<br />

several supporting arguments for this intake hypothesis.<br />

First, as mentioned earlier, there are several studies showing that delaying speech in<br />

second language instruction, when active listening is provided, causes no delay in<br />

attaining proficiency in second language acquisition, <strong>and</strong> may even be beneficial<br />

(for child second language acquisition see Gary, 1975; for adult studies see Asher,<br />

1965, 1966, 1969; Postovsky, 1977). Also, there are suggestive informal accounts<br />

of language acquisition in other cultures, where active listening is stressed. Here is<br />

Sorenson's (1967) report on the American Indians in the Vaupes River area:<br />

The Indians do not practice speaking a language they do not know well yet. Instead,<br />

they passively learn lists of words, forms, phrases in it <strong>and</strong> familiarize themselves<br />

with the sound of its pronunciation... They may make an occasional attempt to speak<br />

a new language in an appropriate situation, but if it does not come easily, they will<br />

not force it.<br />

Finally, there is the well-established fact from child language acquisition studies<br />

that comprehension normally precedes production. Production, in fact, need never<br />

occur. Lenneberg (1962) describes a case of congenital dysarthria in an 8-year-old<br />

boy who never spoke, but who could underst<strong>and</strong> spoken English perfectly well.<br />

Lenneberg noted that:<br />

(A) similar phenomenon in more attenuated form is extremely common.<br />

Underst<strong>and</strong>ing normally precedes speaking by several weeks or months. This<br />

discrepancy is regularly increased in literally all types of developmental speech<br />

disorders <strong>and</strong> is best illustrated in children who have structural deformities in the oral<br />

cavity or pharynx <strong>and</strong> who produce unintelligible speech for years--sometimes<br />

throughout life--without the slightest impairment of underst<strong>and</strong>ing. Congenitally deaf<br />

children also learn to comprehend language in the absence of vocal skills.... However,<br />

there is no clear evidence that speaking is ever present in the absence of<br />

underst<strong>and</strong>ing.<br />

This does not mean that speaking is not of practical importance, <strong>and</strong> it may be the<br />

case that speaking may indirectly promote language acquisition. What may be the<br />

case is that speaking, engaging in conversation, encourages intake. "Eavesdropping"<br />

(Schumann <strong>and</strong> Schumann, 1977) may provide the acquirer with a certain amount<br />

of intake, but actual conversation, in which the acquirer has at least some control of<br />

the topic <strong>and</strong> in which the acquirer's conversational partner<br />

108

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!