20.03.2013 Views

Second Language Acquisition and Second ... - Stephen Krashen

Second Language Acquisition and Second ... - Stephen Krashen

Second Language Acquisition and Second ... - Stephen Krashen

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

the Dulay <strong>and</strong> Burt child second language studies used the BSM, the Bailey et al.<br />

adult study used the BSM, <strong>and</strong> when Larsen-Freeman used the BSM she obtained a<br />

similar order, but when she used other tasks she did not. Also, Porter (1977)<br />

reported that child first language acquirers produced what appeared to be the second<br />

language acquisition order when the BSM was used; as pointed out above, first <strong>and</strong><br />

second language acquisition orders are somewhat different. While this evidence is at<br />

first glance suggestive, recent studies <strong>and</strong> reanalysis show conclusively, I believe,<br />

that the natural order is not an artifact of the BSM. First, we have obtained the<br />

natural order without the BSM, in the composition studies cited above, <strong>and</strong> more<br />

recently in free (spontaneous) speech in a study of adult second language acquirers<br />

(<strong>Krashen</strong>, Houck, Giunchi, Bode, Birnbaum, <strong>and</strong> Strei, 1977). Also, the SLOPE test<br />

gives an order quite similar to that found in the grammatical morpheme studies<br />

(<strong>Krashen</strong> et al., 1976; Fuller, 1978). (Some of these data were obtained <strong>and</strong> reported<br />

on after this objection was raised.) <strong>Second</strong>, Porter's child first language BSM order<br />

is not at all dissimilar to first language orders previously published in the literature<br />

(rho = 0.67 with de Villiers <strong>and</strong> de Villiers, 1973, which just misses the 0.05 level<br />

of significance, quite impressive for a rank order correlation using just seven items).<br />

(For a fuller discussion, see my response to Porter (<strong>Krashen</strong>, 1978c) in <strong>Language</strong><br />

Learning.)<br />

2. Do cross-sectional <strong>and</strong> longitudinal studies agree?<br />

It has been suggested that there is considerable individual variation in morpheme<br />

orders, <strong>and</strong> that longitudinal <strong>and</strong> cross-sectional studies do not always agree<br />

(Rosansky, 1976). In an attempt to determine just how much variation really exists,<br />

I recently reviewed every study available to me where grammatical morphemes<br />

were analyzed in obligatory occasions. This included child L1, child L2, delayed<br />

L1, <strong>and</strong> adult agrammatics. It included both grouped <strong>and</strong> individual studies, <strong>and</strong><br />

longitudinal <strong>and</strong> cross-sectional studies. The complete list of reports consulted is<br />

given in Table 1.<br />

Following de Villiers (1974) I only included morphemes with at least ten obligatory<br />

occasions in a given study. This is an extremely small number, <strong>and</strong> I originally<br />

thought that this would produce large<br />

56

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!