20.07.2013 Views

Beyond Time - Linguistics - University of California, Berkeley

Beyond Time - Linguistics - University of California, Berkeley

Beyond Time - Linguistics - University of California, Berkeley

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Fleisch (2000), and Seidel (2008) demonstrate that past/perfective/anterior morphology<br />

plays such roles in Lucazi (Bantu, K.13) and Yeyi (Bantu, R.41) narratives. While in many<br />

ways specific to narrative, these functions are also consonant with the general meanings<br />

and pragmatic functions <strong>of</strong> the markers, and are <strong>of</strong> use for a holistic understanding <strong>of</strong> their<br />

development and meanings. As Seidel notes, “merely analyzing the tense aspect morphology<br />

and [its] semantic scope in terms <strong>of</strong> single utterances or small texts would lead to a skewed<br />

picture <strong>of</strong> the tense-aspect system” (2008:358).<br />

In her study <strong>of</strong> the Chisukwa (Bantu, M.202) verb and its morphology, Kershner (2002:<br />

231) notes what seems like a “Remote Past marker” -ka- that she analyzes as a marker <strong>of</strong><br />

the dissociative D-domain, as well as an apparent “Immediate Past” marker -aa-, which<br />

according to Kershner, “actually marks a preceding comparable time unit with respect to<br />

the currently understood time unit”; verbs with -aa- are in what Botne & Kershner (2008)<br />

call the P-domain.<br />

In Chisukwa narratives, -ka- occurs when the narrative depicts an event that is “subjectively<br />

distant” or “temporally remote”: examples include “fictional stories” and “personal<br />

histories” (Kershner 2002:232). In these narratives, -ka- marks storyline events, and when<br />

-aa- (and another P-domain marker, -ku-) occur, they give background information. In<br />

contrast, narratives about “current events or economic activities [depicted] as subjectively<br />

near”, -ku- is used for storyline events giving new information; -aa- is still used for background<br />

information (Kershner 2002:232). The behavior <strong>of</strong> the -ka- and -aa- markers in<br />

narratives leads Kershner to conclude that<br />

Although traditional studies have assumed that tense can best be represented<br />

linearly on a timeline, the evidence from narratives has shown that the organization<br />

and semantics <strong>of</strong> these morphemes is more complex and that they are only<br />

superficially tense-like in behavior (Kershner 2002:232, emphasis added).<br />

Fleisch (2000) notes that in Lucazi narratives, the use <strong>of</strong> the Simple Past (-à-. . . - ÌLe)<br />

typically occurs at scene changes, when there is a “change in the set, either a character<br />

coming or going, different location, [or] different time” (Fleisch 2000:286). However, this<br />

scene-changing function is also apparently a matter <strong>of</strong> style. Narratives may also be told<br />

using mostly the Consecutive form (-àkà. . . -à) or the perfective (-à. . . -à), but these are<br />

<strong>of</strong>ten considered more “clumsy”; with the exclusive use <strong>of</strong> the Consecutive is even less “literary”<br />

than the Perfective (Fleisch 2000:263). Respectively substituting the Perfective and<br />

Consecutive for the Simple Past in a sample narrative <strong>of</strong> consecutive actions resulted in<br />

consultant comments that the new texts were “acceptable but bad in style, or sounding as<br />

if the text was taken from everyday language (<strong>of</strong>ten [hinting also] that the events seemed to<br />

have happened in a more recent past)“ (Fleisch 2000:264).<br />

The use <strong>of</strong> the simple past and perfective in Lucazi narrative is also constrained by<br />

situation type. Fleisch divides verbs into three main lexical classes: actions, processes, and<br />

situations. Actions are “dynamic events . . . which are controlled by their logical subjects”<br />

(Fleisch 2000:228). Processes, on the other hand, “do not assign the agent role to their<br />

subject”: they cannot answer the question ‘what did X do?’ (Fleisch 2000:235). These<br />

28

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!