20.07.2013 Views

Beyond Time - Linguistics - University of California, Berkeley

Beyond Time - Linguistics - University of California, Berkeley

Beyond Time - Linguistics - University of California, Berkeley

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

“a new reference frame is opened for a new series <strong>of</strong> events that might only loosely be<br />

connected plotwise to the preceding major episode” (Seidel 2008:385).<br />

4. Introduce important episodes: Resumptive use <strong>of</strong> the P-Anterior may be found at the<br />

beginning <strong>of</strong> major episodes, summarizing the action before it occurs. Seidel analyzes<br />

this use as a stylistic device, breaking the respective episodes from previous events<br />

and emphasizing their importance. Resumptive P-Anterior use <strong>of</strong>ten accompanies a<br />

“major shift in narrative style”, as well (Seidel 2008:382).<br />

Thus, it appears that in Yeyi, the H-Anterior indicates a kind <strong>of</strong> psychological nearness,<br />

while the P-Anterior creates psychological distance from the situation described, consonant<br />

with its temporal function <strong>of</strong> indicating a more distant past.<br />

Finally, it should be noted that temporal-distance distinctions may be different and even<br />

somewhat neutralized in narratives. Kamba (E.50) (Whiteley & Muli 1962, discussed in Dahl<br />

1985:121-122) distinguishes three pasts: a Hodiernal/Immediate past, a Hesternal/Recent<br />

(up to “a week previously”) Past, and a Distant Past. In narratives, however, two different<br />

past tenses are used, and the difference between them is “vaguer”: Narrative II “ ‘connotes<br />

a rather less remote time in the past’ ” than Narrative I (Whiteley & Muli 1962 in Dahl<br />

1985:122).<br />

Kamba and other cases lead Dahl to posit the following generalization:<br />

If narrative and non-narrative contexts differ with respect to the marking <strong>of</strong><br />

temporal distance, it will be the non-narrative contexts that exhibit the largest<br />

number <strong>of</strong> distinctions (Dahl 1985:127)<br />

By way <strong>of</strong> explanation, Dahl posits that since topic time is “by definition” determined by a<br />

narrative’s context, “any further indication <strong>of</strong> its location in time will be redundant” (Dahl<br />

1985:127). Also, narratives may avoid the hodiernal past when it could be conflated with<br />

the perfect (anterior) (Dahl 1985:127).<br />

1.2.6 Motivations for the study <strong>of</strong> tense and aspect pragmatics in<br />

Totela narrative<br />

Bantu languages provide a near ideal source for both synchronic and diachronic studies <strong>of</strong><br />

tense, aspect, and mood, their grammaticalization, and their pragmatics. A typical Bantu<br />

language will have an extremely developed and nuanced system for marking tense and aspect<br />

distinctions, leading Dahl to characterize them as having “the most complex TMA systems in<br />

general” (Dahl 1985:185). For example, Bantu languages are known for expressing multiple<br />

degrees <strong>of</strong> past and future reference. Nurse (2008:20-25) lists other Bantu “innovations”,<br />

such as the “itive” (‘go and X’), multiple patterns <strong>of</strong> negation, a disjunctive/conjunctive<br />

focus opposition, a persistive (‘still’) aspect, and narrative tense.<br />

Furthermore, the approximately 550 Bantu varieties spoken today (Maho 2003, cited in<br />

Nurse 2008:2) are both sufficiently similar and sufficiently different to make comparison valuable.<br />

For example, most have approximately the same aspect “set”, including “perfective,<br />

31

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!