23.02.2013 Views

Mirror-touch synaesthesia: the role of shared ... - UCL Discovery

Mirror-touch synaesthesia: the role of shared ... - UCL Discovery

Mirror-touch synaesthesia: the role of shared ... - UCL Discovery

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

11<br />

Chapter 1<br />

can skip generations (Hubbard and Ramachandran, 2003), and that <strong>the</strong> proportion <strong>of</strong><br />

sons or daughters born to synaes<strong>the</strong>te mo<strong>the</strong>rs does not significantly differ (Barnett et<br />

al., 2008; Ward and Simner, 2005), suggest that an X-linked dominant mode <strong>of</strong><br />

inheritance may be an over simplified account <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> genetic mechanisms which<br />

underlie developmental forms <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> condition (Asher et al., 2009).<br />

Current estimates on <strong>the</strong> prevalence <strong>of</strong> developmental <strong>synaes<strong>the</strong>sia</strong> indicate<br />

that <strong>the</strong> condition has a prevalence rate <strong>of</strong> at least 4% and a female to male ratio <strong>of</strong> 1:1<br />

(Simner et al., 2006; Ward and Simner, 2005). Although, depending on whe<strong>the</strong>r one<br />

includes cases <strong>of</strong> ordinal linguistic personification (in which individuals attribute<br />

genders or personalities to letters or numbers; Simner and Holenstein, 2007) or spatial<br />

number forms (Sagiv, Simner, Collins, Butterworth, and Ward, 2006), <strong>the</strong> prevalence<br />

rate <strong>of</strong> 4% is likely to be much higher (Simner et al., 2006).<br />

A trend <strong>of</strong> all studies <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> prevalence <strong>of</strong> <strong>synaes<strong>the</strong>sia</strong> is to report a higher<br />

proportion <strong>of</strong> synaes<strong>the</strong>tes who experience colour evoked by letters or o<strong>the</strong>r linguistic<br />

stimuli (e.g. grapheme-colour / day-colour <strong>synaes<strong>the</strong>sia</strong>; Baron-Cohen, Burt, Smith-<br />

Laittan, Harrison, and Bolton, 1996; Rich, Bradshaw, and Mattingley, 2005; Simner<br />

et al., 2006). It is perhaps not surprising that this variant <strong>of</strong> <strong>synaes<strong>the</strong>sia</strong> has been <strong>the</strong><br />

topic <strong>of</strong> much research amongst <strong>synaes<strong>the</strong>sia</strong> researchers. Research into this variant<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> condition has highlighted a number <strong>of</strong> interesting individual differences<br />

between synaes<strong>the</strong>tes. For example, distinctions have been made between projector<br />

and associator synaes<strong>the</strong>tes; which distinguishes between synaes<strong>the</strong>tes whose locus <strong>of</strong><br />

experienced colour is projected to a specific spatial location (projector synaes<strong>the</strong>tes)<br />

and synaes<strong>the</strong>tes whose concurrent is perceived internally or in <strong>the</strong> ‘minds eye’<br />

(associator synaes<strong>the</strong>tes) (Dixon, Smilek, and Merilke, 2004; see also Ward, Li, Salih,<br />

and Sagiv, 2007). Similarly, Ramachandran and Hubbard (2001) have categorised

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!