23.02.2013 Views

Mirror-touch synaesthesia: the role of shared ... - UCL Discovery

Mirror-touch synaesthesia: the role of shared ... - UCL Discovery

Mirror-touch synaesthesia: the role of shared ... - UCL Discovery

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

158<br />

Chapter 9<br />

2 also indicate a number <strong>of</strong> features linked to <strong>the</strong> characteristics <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> condition. The<br />

findings from experiment 2 indicate that <strong>the</strong> inducer for synaes<strong>the</strong>tic experience is not<br />

linked to spatial cueing, but is related to bodily <strong>touch</strong> (and in some cases <strong>touch</strong> to<br />

objects). The findings also indicate <strong>the</strong> specular subtype is <strong>the</strong> more common frame<br />

<strong>of</strong> reference adopted by mirror-<strong>touch</strong> synaes<strong>the</strong>tes and <strong>the</strong> relative frequencies<br />

(approximately 81% show a specular frame <strong>of</strong> reference) are similar to those reported<br />

in studies investigating <strong>the</strong> preferred spatial frame adopted when imitating ano<strong>the</strong>r’s<br />

behaviour - both adults and children tend to imitate in a specular mode (Sch<strong>of</strong>ield,<br />

1976; Franz, Ford and Werner, 2007). Fur<strong>the</strong>r characteristics indicate commonalities<br />

between mirror-<strong>touch</strong> <strong>synaes<strong>the</strong>sia</strong> and o<strong>the</strong>r variants <strong>of</strong> <strong>synaes<strong>the</strong>sia</strong>. For example, a<br />

general characteristic <strong>of</strong> <strong>synaes<strong>the</strong>sia</strong> is that different variants <strong>of</strong> <strong>synaes<strong>the</strong>sia</strong> tend to<br />

co-occur (Simner et al., 2006). This also appears to be <strong>the</strong> case in mirror-<strong>touch</strong><br />

<strong>synaes<strong>the</strong>sia</strong>. Synaes<strong>the</strong>tic experiences also tend to be consistent over time (Baron-<br />

Cohen, Wyke, and Binnie, 1987) and <strong>the</strong> mirror-<strong>touch</strong> synaes<strong>the</strong>te’s spatial sub-type<br />

(i.e. whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>y belong to <strong>the</strong> specular or anatomical category) appears to be<br />

consistent across time (Chapter 2) and across different body parts (Banissy and Ward,<br />

2007).<br />

While <strong>the</strong>se findings indicate that mirror-<strong>touch</strong> <strong>synaes<strong>the</strong>sia</strong> shares common<br />

ground with o<strong>the</strong>r types <strong>of</strong> <strong>synaes<strong>the</strong>sia</strong>, possible similarities in <strong>the</strong> neural basis <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

condition are less apparent. A point <strong>of</strong> dispute in <strong>the</strong> <strong>synaes<strong>the</strong>sia</strong> literature is<br />

whe<strong>the</strong>r synaes<strong>the</strong>tic experience is due to cross-activation between brain regions<br />

(ei<strong>the</strong>r through increased structural connectivity or malfunctions in cortical inhibition)<br />

or disinhibition <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> same cortical networks found in non-synaes<strong>the</strong>tes (Bargary and<br />

Mitchell, 2008; Cohen Kadosh, Henik, Catena, Walsh, and Fuetnes, 2009; Cohen<br />

Kadosh and Walsh, 2008; Grossenbacher and Lovelace, 2001; Hubbard and

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!