31.12.2013 Views

Pronomen Abundans and Pronomen Coniunctum. A ... - DWC

Pronomen Abundans and Pronomen Coniunctum. A ... - DWC

Pronomen Abundans and Pronomen Coniunctum. A ... - DWC

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

PRONOMEN ABUNDANS AND PRONOMEN CONIUNCTUM 21<br />

on the word À.éOVTt. The answer, however, mayalso be negative: noticing<br />

how many more cases are found in the Koine than in Ancient Greek - the<br />

difference in number being so great indeed that a comparison between<br />

the two seems almost ridiculous -, one has to admit that in the Koine<br />

something else is going on. The idiom has spread so far that one, while<br />

reading certain texts, can hardly avoid the impression that at that time<br />

people had the idea that each verb had to be supplied with an object<br />

<strong>and</strong> each noun with a possessive genitive, especially when the actual<br />

object or possessor was placed at the beginning ofthe sentence. Apparently<br />

people were not able any longer to span great distances in a sentence<br />

<strong>and</strong> thus put close together everything that belonged to each other,<br />

even when this only could be reached by repetition. Another cause,<br />

closely connected with the preceding one, may have been the shift in<br />

wordorder which began during the period of the Koine. A treatise on this<br />

subject, however, would lead us too far 51.<br />

Let us now return to our question whether this kind ofresumpt. pronoun<br />

<strong>and</strong> the pronomen alJundans are in some way correlated. Blass-Debrunner<br />

associate the two idioms with each other, for they say, speaking of the<br />

pronomen abundans 52 : "Verw<strong>and</strong>t ist das pleonastische Pron. pers. nach<br />

einem Ptz., das einem Relativsatz gleichwertig ist", mentioning as one<br />

of their examples Apoc. 2.17 <strong>and</strong> 17 up Vl"WVTt, M)(J()) avnp. It does not<br />

make any difference, I think, whether the first word of the sentence is a<br />

participle or a noun. It is important that both of them are nouns, st<strong>and</strong>ing<br />

at the beginning of the sentence. And thus we see two idioms: in the one<br />

the senten ce begins with arelat. pronoun, in the other with a noun,<br />

both of them being resumed by a pers. pronoun. The two look very similar,<br />

<strong>and</strong> yet people living at that time do not seem to have seen a correlation<br />

between them. How do we know that? The answer can be very simpie:<br />

if they had seen a correlation between the two, why does the number of<br />

51 See Hjalmar Frisk, Studien zur griechischen Wortstellung, Göteborgs Högskolas<br />

Ársskrift 39 (1933,1). In this study Frisk shows by means of statistical tables that<br />

in the Koine the wordorder object.predicate was preferred, whereas in Ancient<br />

Greek this was exactly the opposite. In classical authors Frisk found for the sequence<br />

predicate-object percentages ranging from 23,9 to 51,6%, but in Polybius 50%<br />

<strong>and</strong> in Matthew even 77,8%. One of the consequences of th is shift may have been<br />

that, when in the Koine for some reason or other the object, instead of being placed<br />

af ter the verb, was put at the beginning of the sentence, people were inclined to<br />

repeat this object af ter the verb by means of a pers. pronoun. There is reason to<br />

think that this supposition is correct, but we cannot be sure until the wordorder<br />

of more Koine-texts has been studied. It is a fact that in texts of the 12th <strong>and</strong><br />

following centuries the sequence predicate-object as weIl as the repetition of the<br />

object by means of a pers. pronoun (in cases where the other sequence is chosen)<br />

appears to have become a rule instead of a tendency. For the situation on this point<br />

in Modern Greek see Tzartzanos, § 84,2, <strong>and</strong> for the phenomenon in general, Bakker,<br />

Ontwikkeling, pp. 170-1.<br />

52 Blass-Debrunner, § 297.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!