23.02.2014 Views

Saddleback Journal of Biology - Saddleback College

Saddleback Journal of Biology - Saddleback College

Saddleback Journal of Biology - Saddleback College

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Spring 2010 <strong>Biology</strong> 3B Paper<br />

Introduction<br />

Largemouth bass (Micropterus salmoides)<br />

are the dominant top carnivores <strong>of</strong> many North<br />

American lakes and reservoirs and are popular sport<br />

fish, but the behavioral mechanisms <strong>of</strong> their feeding<br />

are still poorly known. After reviewing numerous<br />

scientific journals it has become apparent that<br />

largemouth bass have evolved advanced predatory<br />

mechanisms that help them find prey (Moeller, 1972,<br />

Johnke, 1995, Ludsin, 1997,Linser et al, 1998,<br />

Pawson, 2007) though little seems to be known.<br />

Micropterus use their six senses, including their<br />

lateral line system, used for detecting water pressure<br />

variances, to detect prey in all conditions <strong>of</strong> water<br />

(Johnke, 1995). These senses are not only used in<br />

hunting and feeding, but also in evasive maneuvers,<br />

avoiding any temptations that seem less than natural,<br />

such as lures. However, largemouth bass are known<br />

to be attracted to certain colors, shapes, smells, and<br />

vibrations that resemble real life prey which brought<br />

about the invention <strong>of</strong> the common fishing lure<br />

(Brown, 2002). As stated above, largemouth bass<br />

have six senses which mainly benefit them when<br />

living in the murkiest <strong>of</strong> water. The experiment<br />

performed tested what color crank bait lure<br />

largemouth bass prefer in minimal to full daylight<br />

conditions (Linser et al, 1998). Results <strong>of</strong> examining<br />

the most popular line and reel set-up used on average<br />

will help the average fisherman/woman ensure a<br />

good fishing experience in California winter<br />

conditions (Moeller, 1972). It is hypothesized that<br />

the colorful bait in both light conditions will have the<br />

highest frequency <strong>of</strong> successful catches in the allotted<br />

amount <strong>of</strong> experimental time.<br />

Methods<br />

The experiment was conducted at Mission<br />

Viejo Lake in Mission Viejo, California. The fishing<br />

was designated to the end <strong>of</strong> the dock where casting<br />

reached the bass hunting grounds. The lures tested<br />

consisted <strong>of</strong> two main categories: brightly and<br />

neutrally colored crank baits that were used an equal<br />

amount <strong>of</strong> hours. Among the brightly colored crank<br />

baits, the color ranged from neon yellow, to bright<br />

blue. Among the naturally colored baits, the color<br />

ranged from dark brown to grey. All crank baits<br />

were tied with a Palomar knot and right on top <strong>of</strong> the<br />

lure to ensure that the bait reached the average depth<br />

at which largemouth bass typically live (between 7<br />

and 15 feet below the surface). The selection <strong>of</strong> reels<br />

varies from high rate flexibility to little flexibility due<br />

to budget restraints. The reels used were<br />

spinning/casting reels which allowed investigators to<br />

cast out and reel in at a personally customized<br />

selection and pace. The strength <strong>of</strong> the fishing line<br />

used was 6-8 pound tests which made it light enough<br />

to feel the bite <strong>of</strong> the average largemouth bass and<br />

strong enough to reel a largemouth bass in. Over the<br />

course <strong>of</strong> a month, 136 hours <strong>of</strong> fishing, in varying<br />

light conditions, was conducted. Surface water<br />

temperature was recorded as well as detailed<br />

accounts <strong>of</strong> the weather conditions. The data were<br />

collected and analyzed using Micros<strong>of</strong>t Excel. The<br />

data were run through a descriptive statistics test to<br />

determine the standard <strong>of</strong> error <strong>of</strong> means. An<br />

unpaired two-tailed T test assuming equal variance<br />

was run to analyze the data.<br />

Results<br />

The overall number <strong>of</strong> fish caught using the<br />

bright and naturally-colored lures is represented by<br />

figure 1. The two graphs constructed below (Figures<br />

3&4) display the amount <strong>of</strong> fish caught with<br />

naturally-colored and brightly-colored lures in both<br />

low light conditions (sunrise and sundown beyond)<br />

and bright light conditions (between sunrise and<br />

sunset). The last figure is a calendar <strong>of</strong> the moon<br />

phases <strong>of</strong> April 2010. This displays the amount <strong>of</strong><br />

fish caught on each fishing day and it’s accordance to<br />

the major moon phases.<br />

7<br />

6<br />

# <strong>of</strong> Fish Caught<br />

5<br />

4<br />

3<br />

2<br />

1<br />

0<br />

Brightly-colored Lures<br />

Shaking Deep-diving Lures<br />

Naturally-colored Lures<br />

Figure 1. Catch rate <strong>of</strong> Largemouth bass with brightly-colored vs. naturally-colored lures.<br />

48<br />

<strong>Saddleback</strong> <strong>Journal</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Biology</strong><br />

Spring 2010

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!