27.10.2015 Views

What do students know and understand about the Holocaust?

What-do-students-know-and-understand-about-the-Holocaust1

What-do-students-know-and-understand-about-the-Holocaust1

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

38<br />

Collective conceptions of <strong>the</strong> <strong>Holocaust</strong><br />

2006, emphasis added).Mounting empirical evidence<br />

from a variety of different curriculum contexts<br />

suggests that <strong>students</strong> are likely to approach any<br />

‘new’ area of study replete with ‘if not <strong>know</strong>ledge,<br />

<strong>the</strong>n ideas, beliefs, attitudes <strong>and</strong> images in <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

mind’ (Pendry et al. 1997: 20; see also Donovan<br />

<strong>and</strong> Bransford 2005). Some of <strong>the</strong>se attitudes,<br />

beliefs or ideas may be directly at odds with <strong>the</strong><br />

information <strong>and</strong> underst<strong>and</strong>ing that a teacher hopes<br />

to communicate. But as Torney-Purta (1991: 194)<br />

surmises, without an adequate engagement with<br />

<strong>students</strong>’ starting points, ‘<strong>the</strong> presentation of “correct<br />

facts” alone’ is unlikely to be very effective<br />

in ‘dislodging’ <strong>the</strong>se.<br />

With that in mind, this chapter begins its analysis<br />

<strong>and</strong> reporting as a teacher might, by examining<br />

<strong>students</strong>’ awareness of <strong>the</strong> <strong>Holocaust</strong> in <strong>the</strong> broadest<br />

terms. In <strong>do</strong>ing so, it offers a distinctive perspective<br />

from <strong>the</strong> chapters that follow <strong>and</strong> presents its data<br />

through a somewhat wider-angle lens. At this stage<br />

<strong>the</strong> concern is not so much with <strong>the</strong> precise factual<br />

content that individual <strong>students</strong> were able or not to<br />

demonstrate, but ra<strong>the</strong>r with overarching patterns<br />

<strong>and</strong> shared frameworks for underst<strong>and</strong>ing. It is <strong>the</strong>se<br />

patterns <strong>and</strong> framings that are characterised as<br />

<strong>students</strong>’ collective conceptions of <strong>the</strong> <strong>Holocaust</strong>.<br />

Most related literature, at least within <strong>the</strong> field<br />

of history education, employs a vocabulary of<br />

‘preconceptions’ <strong>and</strong>/or ‘misconception’, <strong>and</strong><br />

focuses on <strong>the</strong> individual as its primary point of<br />

reference (see, for example, Epstein 2012; Gray<br />

2011; Conway 2006; Husb<strong>and</strong>s <strong>and</strong> Pendry 2000;<br />

Pendry et al. 1997). There are three important<br />

distinctions in <strong>the</strong> approach outlined here:<br />

1. Our research draws on responses offered by<br />

<strong>students</strong> from across all seven years of secondary<br />

education, <strong>the</strong> majority of whom had already<br />

encountered <strong>the</strong> <strong>Holocaust</strong> on at least one<br />

occasion within school. As a consequence, it<br />

would be both misleading <strong>and</strong> unhelpful to claim<br />

access to a ‘starting point’ in <strong>students</strong>’ thinking,<br />

to a point that is prior to a specific educational<br />

intervention, as <strong>the</strong> term ‘preconception’ implies.<br />

2. The term ‘misconception’ specifically denotes<br />

inaccuracies or limitations in student thinking.<br />

While relationships between <strong>students</strong>’ ideas <strong>about</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> <strong>Holocaust</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> existing historical record will<br />

be critically examined in later chapters, this is not<br />

<strong>the</strong> primary focus of analysis here.<br />

3. Ra<strong>the</strong>r than focusing on individuals, this chapter<br />

is centrally concerned with shared forms of<br />

consciousness <strong>and</strong> with overarching patterns.<br />

For <strong>the</strong>se reasons a <strong>the</strong>oretical framing that<br />

emphasises social ra<strong>the</strong>r than individualised<br />

cognition has been applied.<br />

The notion of ‘schema’ or ‘schemata’, as an<br />

organising structure for thoughts, plays a central role<br />

in <strong>the</strong> analysis that follows, for it is not just individual<br />

beliefs <strong>and</strong> ideas that are important but <strong>the</strong> manner<br />

in which a collection of thoughts become interrelated<br />

<strong>and</strong> arranged. As Wertsch (2002: 7) describes:<br />

<strong>the</strong> past several decades of research in <strong>the</strong><br />

psychology of memory… [have] shown time <strong>and</strong><br />

again memory is more a matter of organising, or<br />

reconstructing, bits of information into a general<br />

scheme than it is a matter of accurate recall of <strong>the</strong><br />

isolated bits <strong>the</strong>mselves.<br />

This in turn can have a profound impact on our<br />

underst<strong>and</strong>ing of <strong>students</strong>’ learning or, at <strong>the</strong> very<br />

least, of <strong>the</strong>ir information recall.<br />

In much recent educational <strong>the</strong>ory, <strong>the</strong> term<br />

‘schema’ is used as though it denotes something<br />

akin to a storage facility that freezes <strong>and</strong> fixes<br />

information inside an individual’s head. However,<br />

Frederic Bartlett, <strong>the</strong> English psychologist who<br />

first developed <strong>the</strong> concept, thought of memory in<br />

‘embodied, dynamic, temporal, holistic, <strong>and</strong> social’<br />

terms (Wagoner 2013: 553, emphasis added).<br />

Bartlett (1932) did not believe that ‘remembering’<br />

happened exclusively – nor even primarily – ‘in <strong>the</strong><br />

head’ but ra<strong>the</strong>r through people’s ongoing interaction<br />

with each o<strong>the</strong>r <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir worlds.<br />

It is this socially situated underst<strong>and</strong>ing of memory<br />

<strong>and</strong> cognition that we want to attend to here. For, as<br />

Chapter 1 has already argued, ‘<strong>the</strong> <strong>Holocaust</strong>’ is not<br />

only an object of historical enquiry: in recent years<br />

it has also been constructed <strong>and</strong> communicated as<br />

a sociocultural phenomenon <strong>and</strong> powerful symbolic<br />

resource (see Alex<strong>and</strong>er 2003; Cole 1999; Novick<br />

1999). Indeed, ‘all <strong>know</strong>ledge of <strong>the</strong> past is social<br />

<strong>know</strong>ledge’ (Uzell <strong>and</strong> Blud 1993: 131) <strong>and</strong> is<br />

<strong>the</strong>refore both informed by, <strong>and</strong> in turn reflects,<br />

many of <strong>the</strong> perspectives <strong>and</strong> priorities shared by<br />

wider society.<br />

A related <strong>the</strong>oretical framework is provided by<br />

<strong>the</strong> Romanian social psychologist Serge Moscovici<br />

(1981, 1984) who employed <strong>the</strong> term ‘social<br />

representations’ to describe ‘collective systems of<br />

meaning’ (Duveen <strong>and</strong> Lloyd 1993); ‘<strong>know</strong>ledge<br />

<strong>and</strong> information which people share in <strong>the</strong> form of<br />

common-sense <strong>the</strong>ories <strong>about</strong> <strong>the</strong> social world’<br />

(Augoustinos <strong>and</strong> Innes 1990: 215). While <strong>the</strong> two<br />

terms, ‘schemata’ <strong>and</strong> ‘social representations’ are<br />

far from interchangeable, <strong>the</strong>re are a number of<br />

important points of intersection between Bartlett<br />

<strong>and</strong> Moscovici’s perspectives which are relevant<br />

to discussion here.<br />

In different ways both Bartlett <strong>and</strong> Moscovici<br />

characterise memory as an ‘effort after meaning’<br />

(Wagoner 2011: 106). From this perspective,<br />

<strong>know</strong>ledge-acquisition is never a passive process:

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!