12.07.2015 Views

Télécharger le texte intégral

Télécharger le texte intégral

Télécharger le texte intégral

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

2 MILLET ET AL.Downloaded By: [Mil<strong>le</strong>t, Xavier] At: 10:57 4 November 2009re<strong>le</strong>vance of such distinction in AD and have reported thatAD patients exhibited normal priming in identificationtasks and diminished priming performances in productiontasks (F<strong>le</strong>ischman et al., 2001; Gabrieli et al., 1999).Among the variety of tasks used to investigate implicitmemory in normal aging and AD, word-stem comp<strong>le</strong>tionpriming (Warrington & Weiskrantz, 1974) is incontestablythe most frequently used. In this task,participants are given a three-<strong>le</strong>tter beginning word-stemand are asked to evoke the first word that comes to mind.A priming effect is ref<strong>le</strong>cted by a higher probability tocomp<strong>le</strong>te the word stems with the previously studiedwords than with new (unstudied) words. According tothe perceptual–conceptual distinction, word-stemcomp<strong>le</strong>tion that is perceptually driven should be consideredas a perceptual priming task (Roediger et al., 1989)and thus should not be affected in AD. On the contrary,the identification–production distinction would predictimpaired word-stem comp<strong>le</strong>tion priming in AD since thetask requires production processes involving the competitionbetween multip<strong>le</strong> alternative responses (F<strong>le</strong>ischmanet al., 2001; Gabrieli et al., 1999). Regarding the availab<strong>le</strong>results, the number of studies reporting reducedword-stem comp<strong>le</strong>tion priming and the number ofstudies reporting no impairment appear approximatelyequiva<strong>le</strong>nt. In attempts to clarify such discrepancy, twometa-analyses assessed the integrity of word-stemcomp<strong>le</strong>tion priming in AD (Meiran & Jelicic, 1995;Russo & Spinn<strong>le</strong>r, 1994). Both concluded that wordstempriming is affected in AD. However, followingthese meta-analyses, numerous studies provided inconsistentresults (Arroyo-Anllo, Ingrand, Neau, Aireault,& Gil, 2004; Beauregard, Chertkow, Gold, & Bergman,2001; Cacho Gutierrez, Garcia Garcia, & FernandezCalvo, 2000; Camus et al., 2003; Car<strong>le</strong>simo et al., 1999;Deweer et al., 1994; Downes et al., 1996; F<strong>le</strong>ischmanet al., 1997; Koivisto, Portin, Seinela, & Rinne, 1998;Kuzis et al., 1999; Park et al., 1998; Pasquier, Grymonprez,Lebert, & Van der Linden, 2001).Methodological variations have been proposed toexplain the discrepancy of word-stem comp<strong>le</strong>tionpriming results in AD (F<strong>le</strong>ischman & Gabrieli, 1998). Inparticular, variability in encoding conditions could be agood candidate. Indeed, whi<strong>le</strong> word-stem comp<strong>le</strong>tionstudies in AD generally share equiva<strong>le</strong>nt implicit testphaseprocedures, the instructions delivered at encodingwere considerably different. The participants could berequested to respond whether the word contains a particular<strong>le</strong>tter (Gabrieli et al., 1994, 1999), to pronouncethe <strong>le</strong>tter corresponding to a numerical position (Scott,Wright, Rai, Extern-Smith, & Gardiner, 1991), or tocount the number of vowels (Car<strong>le</strong>simo et al., 1999) orsyllab<strong>le</strong>s (Kuzis et al., 1999) within the target word.Encoding phase instructions could also <strong>le</strong>ad participantsto read words aloud (Dick, Kean, & Sands, 1989;Downes et al., 1996; F<strong>le</strong>ischman et al., 1997; Gabrieli et al.,1999; Huberman, Moscovitch, & Freedman, 1994;Keane et al., 1991; Koivisto et al., 1998; LaVoie &Faulkner, 2008) or si<strong>le</strong>ntly (Beauregard et al., 2001) or torate on a sca<strong>le</strong> whether they liked or disliked a word orwhether the words are p<strong>le</strong>asant or unp<strong>le</strong>asant (Bäckman,Almkvist, Nyberg, & Andersson, 2000; Bondi &Kaszniak, 1991; Car<strong>le</strong>simo et al., 1998, 1999; F<strong>le</strong>ischmanet al., 1999; Pasquier et al., 2001; Randolph, 1991;Shimamura, Salmon, Squire, & Butters, 1987). Otherconditions putting emphasis on the meaning of the wordrequired participants to generate words from a givendefinition (F<strong>le</strong>ischman et al., 1999; F<strong>le</strong>ischman et al.,1997) or from a related word (Scott et al., 1991), or tocomp<strong>le</strong>te a sentence with sing<strong>le</strong> word endings (Grosse,Wilson, & Fox, 1990). Moreover, participants could alsobe requested to provide correct definition of words(Burke, Knight, & Partridge, 1994; Car<strong>le</strong>simo et al.,1999; McGeorge, Taylor, Della Sala, & Shanks, 2002;Partridge, Knight, & Feehan, 1990; Russo & Spinn<strong>le</strong>r,1994) or answer “yes or no” to questions relying onsemantic know<strong>le</strong>dge on a particular word (Beauregardet al., 2001; Gabrieli et al., 1994, 1999).As may be seen, the instructions given to participantsat the encoding phase are particularly heterogeneous. Itcan be hypothesized that the nature of processes thatoperate during encoding influences the magnitude oflater implicit retrieval in AD patients. The effect ofencoding manipulation on implicit memory tasks hasbeen widely debated with numerous studies concludingthat litt<strong>le</strong> or no effect of <strong>le</strong>vel of processing could bereported in comp<strong>le</strong>tion priming tasks (e.g., Challis,Velichkovsky, & Craik, 1996; Graf, Mand<strong>le</strong>r, & Haden,1982; Roediger, Weldon, Stad<strong>le</strong>r, & Rieg<strong>le</strong>r, 1992).Some studies in AD reported similar results (Beauregardet al., 2001; Burke et al., 1994; F<strong>le</strong>ischman et al., 1999;F<strong>le</strong>ischman et al., 1997; Russo & Spinn<strong>le</strong>r, 1994; Scott et al.,1991). On the other hand, a significant effect of conceptualself-generation processes has been repeatedlyreported on word-fragment (Gardiner, 1988; Schwartz,1989) and word-stem (Bassili, Smith, & MacLeod, 1989;Car<strong>le</strong>simo, 1994; Car<strong>le</strong>simo et al., 1999) comp<strong>le</strong>tiontasks. Thus, word-stem comp<strong>le</strong>tion priming could alsobe regarded as a conceptual priming task. Moreover,when meta-analyses on implicit memory tasks includingword-stem comp<strong>le</strong>tion priming consider encodingmanipulation, a constant advantage of deep over shallowprocessing is reported (Brown & Mitchell, 1994;Challis & Brodbeck, 1992). In AD, some studies evidencingnormal word-stem comp<strong>le</strong>tion priming performancesfollowing semantic generation supported the conclusionthat an elaborate conceptual encoding condition may benecessary for AD patients to facilitate word-stem comp<strong>le</strong>tionpriming (Grosse et al., 1990; Partridge et al.,1990). Moreover, Maki and Knopman (1996) evidencedthat AD patients exhibited intact generation primingonly in the situation involving elaborative conceptualgeneration of items whatever the perceptual or conceptualnature of the retrieval procedure. The authors concludedfor the necessity of reinstating at retrieval thespecific operation initially required at encoding phase toimprove priming performances in AD, consistently withthe latest model by Roediger, Gallo, and Geraci (2002)providing a more procedural approach of memory.Thus, it can be hypothesized that considering the natureof encoding condition could contribute to clarify wordstemcomp<strong>le</strong>tion priming performances in AD.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!