12.07.2015 Views

Télécharger le texte intégral

Télécharger le texte intégral

Télécharger le texte intégral

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

788 X. Mil<strong>le</strong>t et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 24 (2009) 783–789the increasing comp<strong>le</strong>xity of figures suggesting that they preferentially rely on holistic spatially oriented strategy allowing themrotating the figure as a who<strong>le</strong> in a unitary process. On the contrary, in women, the time to operate mental rotation increased withfigures comp<strong>le</strong>xity suggesting that they preferentially relied on a verbally mediated analytic strategy in which the figure isparsed into individual pieces rotated separately. In neuroimaging studies, this gender-related strategic differentiation hasbeen proposed to explain the results showing that men exhibit greater parietal areas activation, whereas women exhibitlower activation of the parietal lobe but additional activation of the frontal lobe whi<strong>le</strong> performing a mental rotation task(Schöning et al., 2007; Thomsen et al., 2000; Weiss et al., 2003b). Another hypothesis explaining gender effects on visuospatialperformances relies on the relationship between androgens and visuospatial memory. Indeed, studies with animal models haveshown that spatial memory is impaired after gonadectomy (Frye et al., 2001) and that it is restored with androgen adjunction(Edinger & Frye, 2004). Epidemiological studies also showed that lower testosterone <strong>le</strong>vels in elderly men were associatedwith worse spatial memory (Moffat et al., 2002). On the other hand, testosterone adjunction increases spatial cognition inelderly men (Janowsky et al., 1994), fema<strong>le</strong>-to-ma<strong>le</strong> transsexuals (Van Goozen, Cohen-Kettenis, Gooren, Frijda, & Van dePoll, 1994), and hypogonadal men (Gray et al., 2005).Whether gender influences visuospatial deficits in AD may be interesting to consider in the context of the “cognitivereserve” hypothesis (Stern, 2006, for a review). Besides lifesty<strong>le</strong> factors, such as education, occupational attainment (e.g.,Stern et al., 1994), or stimulating <strong>le</strong>isure activities (e.g., Scarmeas, Levy, Tang, Manly, & Stern, 2001), sex was alsoshown to modulate the probability of developing AD, with women having higher risk than men (e.g., Letenneur et al.,1999). In addition to distinct probabilities of incidence, the clinical manifestation of the disease may not be exactly thesame in men in women, since as may be seen in the present study, at equiva<strong>le</strong>nt <strong>le</strong>vel of dementia severity, ma<strong>le</strong> andfema<strong>le</strong> AD patients exhibit different <strong>le</strong>vel of performances in specific dimensions of cognitive functioning. Therefore,despite the physiopathological process of AD irremediably affecting hippocampal and parietal regions, sex may contributeto the constitution of individual cognitive reserve capacity likely to modulate the clinical exteriorization of the disease.Conflict of InterestNone declared.ReferencesAgniel, A., Joanette, Y., Doyon, B., & Duchein, C. (1992). Protoco<strong>le</strong> d’évaluation des gnosies visuel<strong>le</strong>s Montréal-Toulouse. Isbergues: L’OrthoEdition.Cherrier, M. M., Matsumoto, A. M., Amory, J. K., Asthana, S., Bremner, W., Peskind, E. R., et al. (2005). Testosterone improves spatial memory in men withAlzheimer disease and mild cognitive impairment. Neurology, 64, 2063–2068.Collins, D. W., & Kimura, D. (1997). A large sex difference on a two-dimensional mental rotation task. Behavioral Neuroscience, 111, 845–849.Cornoldi, C., & Vecchi, T. (2003). Visuo-spatial working memory and individual differences. Hove: Psychology Press.Corsi, P. M. (1972). Human memory and the medial temporal region of the brain. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, McGill University, Montreal.De Frias, C., Nilsson, L. G., & Herlitz, A. (2006). Sex differences in cognition are stab<strong>le</strong> over a 10-year period in adulthood and old age. Aging,Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 13, 574–587.Della Sala, S., Gray, C., Badde<strong>le</strong>y, A., Allamano, N., & Wilson, L. (1999). Pattern span: A tool for unwelding visuo-spatial memory. Neuropsychologia, 37,1189–1199.Edinger, K. L., & Frye, C. A. (2004). Testosterone’s analgesic, anxiolytic, and cognitive-enhancing effects may be due in part to actions of its 5alpha-reducedmetabolites in the hippocampus. Behavioral Neuroscience, 118, 1352–1364.Folstein, M., Folstein, S., & McHugh, P. (1975). Mini-Mental State. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal ofPsychiatric Research, 12, 189–198.Frye, C. A., Park, D., Tanaka, M., Rosellini, R., & Svare, B. (2001). The testosterone metabolite and neurosteroid 3a-androstanediol may mediate the effects oftestosterone on conditioned place preference. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 26, 731–750.Gaestel, Y., Amieva, H., Letenneur, L., Dartigues, J. F., & Fabrigou<strong>le</strong>, C. (2006). Cube drawing performances in normal ageing and Alzheimer’s disease: Datafrom the PAQUID elderly population-based cohort. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 21, 22–32.Geiser, C., Lehmann, W., Corth, M., & Eid, M. (2008). Quantitative and qualitative change in children’s mental rotation performance. Learning and IndividualDifferences, 18, 419–429.Gray, P. B., Singh, A. B., Woodhouse, L. J., Storer, T. W., Casaburi, R., Dzekov, J., et al. (2005). Dose-dependent effects of testosterone on sexual function,mood, and visuospatial cognition in older men. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 90, 3838–3846.Grossi, D., Becker, J. T., Smith, C., & Trojano, L. (1993). Memory for visuospatial patterns in Alzheimer’s disease. Psychological Medicine, 23, 65–70.Heil, M., & Jansen-Osmann, P. (2008). Sex differences in mental rotation with polygons of different comp<strong>le</strong>xity: Do men utilize holistic processes whereaswomen prefer piecemeal ones? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 683–689.Janowsky, J. S., Oviatt, S. K., & Orwoll, E. S. (1994). Testosterone influences spatial cognition in older men. Behavioral Neuroscience, 108, 325–332.Kurylo, D. D., Allan, W. C., Collins, T. E., & Baron, J. (2003). Perceptual organization based upon spatial relationships in Alzheimer’s disease. BehavioralNeurology, 14, 19–28.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!