07.12.2012 Views

Neural Correlates of Processing Syntax in Music and ... - PubMan

Neural Correlates of Processing Syntax in Music and ... - PubMan

Neural Correlates of Processing Syntax in Music and ... - PubMan

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

<strong>Music</strong> Perception 9<br />

at 5 years <strong>of</strong> age. Trehub, Thorpe, <strong>and</strong> Tra<strong>in</strong>or (1990) demonstrated that 7- to 10-month<br />

old <strong>in</strong>fants detected a change <strong>in</strong> typical Western melodies (based on the major triad)<br />

more readily compared to a change <strong>in</strong> non-typical melodies. However, musical regularities<br />

may be present even <strong>in</strong> much younger children, specifically if implicit measures –<br />

that are more sensitive than explicit measures – are used (Schellenberg, Big<strong>and</strong>, Poul<strong>in</strong>-<br />

Charronnat, Garnier, & Stevens, 2005). Trehub, Thorpe <strong>and</strong> Bull (1990; cited after<br />

Jusczyk & Krumhansl, 1993) showed that 8½-month old <strong>in</strong>fants were more likely to<br />

f<strong>in</strong>d one-semitone changes <strong>in</strong> a melody based on the dom<strong>in</strong>ant-tonic progression than <strong>in</strong><br />

other melodies. This <strong>in</strong>dicates a very early sensitivity to phrase structure, s<strong>in</strong>ce the<br />

dom<strong>in</strong>ant-tonic progression is a prom<strong>in</strong>ent marker for the end <strong>of</strong> a musical phrase. This<br />

sensitivity may help to acquire tonal structure.<br />

The acquisition <strong>of</strong> tonal <strong>and</strong> harmonic regularities might also be <strong>in</strong>fluenced by co<strong>in</strong>cidence<br />

<strong>of</strong> music-structurally important events with metrical accents (Schmuckler &<br />

Boltz, 1994) <strong>and</strong> acoustical characteristics. The hierarchy <strong>of</strong> prom<strong>in</strong>ence <strong>of</strong> tones (or<br />

chords) with<strong>in</strong> a key is – at least to some degree – related to consonance (i.e., simple<br />

frequency ratios <strong>and</strong> roughness). Successive tones that are related by simple frequency<br />

ratios were processed more readily (Schellenberg & Trehub, 1994). Acoustical surface<br />

characteristics are also evidenced to <strong>in</strong>fluence the acquisition <strong>of</strong> regularities among<br />

nonadjacent elements: Creel, Newport, <strong>and</strong> Asl<strong>in</strong> (2004) demonstrated that (adult)<br />

learners did not acquire highly consistent statistical regularities among nonadjacent<br />

tones when these were similar <strong>in</strong> pitch range <strong>and</strong> timbre. In contrast, these regularities<br />

were acquired when the tones shared an acoustic property (such as pitch range or timbre).<br />

However, listeners may also implicitly learn statistical regularities un<strong>in</strong>fluenced by<br />

acoustical surface characteristics (as different timbre; Tillmann & McAdams, 2004).<br />

Moreover, <strong>in</strong>fants <strong>and</strong> adults might weight conflict<strong>in</strong>g acoustic cues differently <strong>and</strong><br />

experience with Western music may <strong>in</strong>crease culturally biased respond<strong>in</strong>g, thereby<br />

decreas<strong>in</strong>g effects <strong>of</strong> acoustical characteristics.<br />

<strong>Music</strong> is highly effective <strong>in</strong> regulat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> optimiz<strong>in</strong>g mood <strong>and</strong> convey<strong>in</strong>g emotion <strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>fants, adolescents <strong>and</strong> adults (Trehub, 2003d). In children tone quality elicits the<br />

strongest early reactions: smooth, treble-registered sounds elicit attention <strong>and</strong> pleasure.<br />

Children below 5 years seem not to differentiate emotionally between music <strong>in</strong> conventional<br />

compared to dissonant harmony (Moog, 1976; Schellenberg & Tra<strong>in</strong>or, 1996;<br />

Schellenberg & Trehub, 1994). In Western music, the mood <strong>of</strong> a certa<strong>in</strong> piece is related<br />

to its mode (i.e., pieces <strong>in</strong> major key are usually perceived as more happy than pieces <strong>in</strong><br />

m<strong>in</strong>or key); this relation has to be acquired by children (Gerardi & Gerken, 1995).<br />

Tempo may further <strong>in</strong>fluence the perception <strong>of</strong> the affective value <strong>of</strong> musical pieces.<br />

Dalla Bella, Peretz, Rousseau, <strong>and</strong> Gossel<strong>in</strong> (2001) asked children to determ<strong>in</strong>e the

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!